Deliberate induction of alcohol tolerance: empirical introduction to a novel health risk (2024)

  • Journal List
  • HHS Author Manuscripts
  • PMC4708259

As a library, NLM provides access to scientific literature. Inclusion in an NLM database does not imply endorsem*nt of, or agreement with, the contents by NLM or the National Institutes of Health.
Learn more: PMC Disclaimer | PMC Copyright Notice

Deliberate induction of alcohol tolerance: empirical introduction to a novel health risk (1)

About Author manuscriptsSubmit a manuscriptHHS Public Access; Author Manuscript; Accepted for publication in peer reviewed journal;

Addiction. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2016 Jan 11.

Published in final edited form as:

Addiction. 2010 Oct; 105(10): 1767–1770.

doi:10.1111/j.1360-0443.2010.03042.x

PMCID: PMC4708259

NIHMSID: NIHMS746380

PMID: 20840199

Julia A. Martinez, Douglas Steinley, and Kenneth J. Sher

The publisher's final edited version of this article is available at Addiction

Abstract

Aims

Alcohol tolerance is a hallmark indicator of alcohol dependence. Even so, the allure of peers’ admiration for having the ability to drink heavily may lead some adolescents and young adults to practice, or ‘train’, to increase their tolerance (particularly at US colleges, where heavy drinking is highly prevalent and central to the social culture). This is a potential health hazard that has not been documented empirically. Thus, we initiated a study of tolerance ‘training’ and its association to risky and heavy drinking.

Design, setting and participants

A cross-sectional online survey of 990 college student life-time drinkers at a large Midwestern US university.

Findings

Of the sample, 9.9% (n = 97) reported deliberately ‘training’ to increase tolerance. On average, they reported increasing from approximately seven to 10 US standard drinks in a night prior to ‘training’ to 12–15 drinks at the end of ‘training,’ over approximately 2–3 weeks’ duration. Although the proportion of frequent binge drinking among ‘non-trainers’ (34.4%) was similar to national rates, ‘trainers’ were much more likely to be frequent bingers (76.3%; OR = 6.15).

Conclusions

A number of students report deliberately inducing alcohol tolerance, probably directly increasing the risk for alcohol poisoning and other acute harms and/or dependence. This phenomenon might additionally be applicable to other populations, and deserves further study and attention as a potential personal and public health risk. Prevention efforts might aim to reduce the perceived importance of heavy-drinking abilities.

Keywords: Alcohol dependence, heavy drinking, symptom, tolerance, training

INTRODUCTION

Alcohol tolerance, defined as (1) the need for noticeably increased amounts of alcohol in order to achieve the desired effect or (2) diminished effect with continued use of the same amount, is a hallmark diagnostic criterion of alcohol dependence [12] and is associated with myriad health risks (e.g. blackouts, morning drinking [3]). Because alcohol tolerance has such negative implications, it is not viewed typically as an attribute that individuals would seek out voluntarily.

Nevertheless, some individuals may pursue acquiring tolerance. In many US colleges, heavy drinking is highly prevalent [4] and often associated with many harmful or even fatal outcomes [5]. However, heavy consumption is sometimes viewed as a social asset, or a way to earn peer admiration via being able to drink more than others before feeling the effects [6]. Thus, rather than working to avoid tolerance, students may practice, or ‘train’ (as an athlete trains for a competitive event), to increase their tolerance for alcohol at heavy drinking events.

Such ‘training’ has not yet been studied empirically, although some entertainment media make reference to the phenomenon [7]. Even so, ‘training’, if it occurred with any frequency in the population, could pose a harmful personal and public health risk. Thus, we initiated an empirical investigation in a sample of 990 US college student drinkers by surveying whether they ever engaged in this behavior. We also surveyed them about the specifics of ‘training’, and we estimated its association with heavy and risky drinking.

METHODS

Participants and procedure

Following approval from the university Institutional Review Board, we offered credit for participating in an online survey to all students enrolled in introductory psychology at a large Midwestern US university. This course has the highest enrollment of any course on campus [8]. A total of 1069 unique individuals consented and completed the survey. We excluded individuals reporting that they never drank any alcohol in their lives (n = 79), yielding a sample of 990. Consistent with the overall introductory psychology class, this sample averaged 18.70 [standard deviation (SD) 0.92; range 18–24] years of age, was 80.5% first-year, 54.5% female and 86.8% white/non-Hispanic. The sample is similar to the university with regard to sex and race [8].

Measures and analyses

We asked students: ‘in the past 12 months have you ever prepared (“trained”) for a future heavy-drinking event by drinking more and/or more frequently than normal in the days leading up to the event in order to improve tolerance?’ (0 = no, 1 = yes). Students indicated how many (US) standard drinks they could drink in a night at both the beginning and end of ‘training’ (1 = one drink, 2 = two drinks, 3 = three–four drinks, 4 = five–six drinks, 5 = seven–eight drinks, 6 = nine–11 drinks, 7 = 12–15 drinks, 8 = 16–18 drinks, 9 = 19–24 drinks, 10 = 25 or more drinks), the duration of time it took them to ‘train’ to their reported tolerance level (1 = 1 week, 2 = 2 weeks, 3 = 3 weeks, 4 = 4 weeks or 1 month, 5 = more than 4 weeks or 1 month), and whether they used each of three types of alcohol (beer, wine, hard liquor: 0 = no, 1 = yes). Also, students indicated the occasions for which they ‘trained:’ a Friday night/weekend, spring break (i.e. vacation/holiday in springtime), end of the school year, (American) football game, birthday, Homecoming, graduation, New Year’s eve, Halloween, St Patrick’s day, Superbowl (i.e. famous American football tournament), wedding, my 21st birthday and (American) Independence day (0 = no, 1 = yes). Students indicated how true it was for them that they ‘trained’ for each of these reasons: I didn’t want to pass out early in the celebration; I wanted to keep up with others’ drinking; I didn’t want to have to think about the amount that I drank and whether it would affect me; I wanted to break my own record; I wanted to impress my friends (0 = not at all true, 1 = a little true, 2 = moderately true, 3 = quite true, 4 = extremely true). Open fields invited respondents’ comments [other ‘training’ events: beer pong tournament, beer olympics, drinking contest, beer fest, formal (dance), senior trip, cruise, float (boating) trip, vacation. another reason for ‘training’: ‘I wanted to win beer fest’].

We estimated whether ‘training’ was associated with sex, race and fraternity/sorority (i.e. Greek) membership (as drinking is comparatively high in males, whites and Greek members [9]). We used logistic regression to estimate the likelihood of ‘training’ from these (dichotomous) variables.

We also estimated the relation between ‘training’ and heavy/risky drinking (using logistic regression). Frequent binge drinking was assessed by reports of having had five or more (males) or four or more (females) (US) standard alcoholic drinks in within a 2-hour period as often as once a week or more in the past 12 months. Frequently getting drunk was assessed by reports of getting drunk on alcohol as often as once a week or more in the past 12 months. The consumption of 12 or more (US) standard alcoholic drinks within a 24-hour period during the life-time was assessed. Whether students ever ‘booted and rallied’, or vomited after drinking alcohol and immediately started drinking again in the life-time, was also assessed (all drinking variable scales: 0 = no, 1 = yes).

RESULTS

Ninety-seven individuals (9.9% of the sample) reported having ‘trained’. Men were more likely than women to ‘train’ [odds ratio (OR) = 3.68, 95% confidence interval (CI) = 2.31–5.89], and Greeks were more likely than non-Greeks to ‘train’ (OR = 1.78, 95% CI = 1.17–2.73). There were no group differences in ‘training’ status as a function of age, class year or race. During ‘training’, students reported increasing drinking from approximately seven–10 to 12–15 standard drinks in a night over approximately 2–3 weeks (t 90 = 11.90, P < 0.001); for women, five–six to nine–11 (t 21 = 9.18, P < 0.001) and for men, eight–11 to 12–17 (t 68 = 9.37, P < 0.001).

Table 1 shows additional characteristics of ‘trainers’. For example, individuals endorsed ‘training’ for a number of events and reasons, with a Friday night/weekend being the most popular event, and not wanting to pass out too early being the most popular reason. Individuals reported ‘training’ for an average of three events (range = 0–13, where 22.7% reported ‘training’ for just one event and 67.0% ‘trained’ for three or fewer). Although men were more likely to ‘train’ in general, they were less likely than women to ‘train’ for a birthday (OR = 0.33, 95% CI = 0.13–0.89) or for spring break (OR = 0.37, 95% CI = 0.14–0.93), social events often associated with extremely high consumption [1011]. These were the only sex differences in events or reasons for ‘training’, and there were no race differences. Greeks, however, were more likely than non-Greeks to ‘train’ for Superbowl (OR = 7.73, 95% CI = 1.59–37.52), a football game (OR = 4.05, 95% CI = 1.54–10.64), birthday (OR = 3.68, 95% CI = 1.40–9.69), spring break (OR = 3.32, 95% CI = 1.36–8.12) and Homecoming (OR = 2.72, 95% CI = 1.01–7.28).

Table 1

‘Training’ to increase alcohol tolerance (n = 97).

% (versus non-‘trainers,’ n = 884)ORindividual
Demographic characteristics:
 Under legal drinking age (i.e. 21 years)93.8 (versus 95.5)0.72
 Male73.2 (versus 42.6)3.68*
 White/non-Hispanic92.7 (versus 86.5)1.22
 Greek (fraternity/sorority) member44.8 (versus 31.3)1.78*
‘Trained’ with:
 Beer78.4
 Hard liquor47.4
 Wine12.4
‘Trained’ for the following event:
 A Friday night/weekend54.6
 Spring break33.0
 End of the school year32.0
Football game (US)26.8
 Birthday25.8
 Homecoming22.7
 Graduation22.7
 New Year’s Eve22.7
 Halloween18.6
 St Patrick’s day17.5
 Superbowl (US football tournament)12.4
 Wedding10.3
 My 21st birthday9.3
 (US) Independence Day5.2
‘Trained’ becausea:
 Didn’t want to pass out early83.5
 Wanted to keep up with others’ drinking81.8
 Didn’t want to have to think about amount75.8
 Wanted to break my own record73.6
 Wanted to impress my friends67.0
Drinking variables
 Frequent binge drinking76.3 (versus 34.4)6.15*
 Frequent getting drunk80.2 (versus 36.2)7.14*
 12 drinks or more as a lifetime max83.5 (versus 45.8)5.99*
 ‘Booted and rallied’ 1+ times in life-time78.4 (versus 32.8)7.40*

Open in a separate window

aPercentages are on a dichotomization of the ordinal scale where 0 = not at all true, 1 = true, and percentages reflect ‘true ’. ‘Booted and rallied’ is a colloquialism for continuing to drink after vomiting. All drinking variables: 0 = no, 1 = yes.

*P < 0.01.

OR: odds ratio.

Table 1 also shows that ‘trainers’ are far more at risk than their counterparts for heavy and risky drinking. Notably, the rate of frequent binge drinking in non-‘trainers’ (34.4%) is roughly comparable to the 2-week prevalence of binge drinking in a nationally representative sample of full-time college students (40.0%) [4], while the rate is much higher in ‘trainers’ (76.3%). Differences hold when statistically controlling for sex, race and Greek membership.

DISCUSSION

Our findings indicate that ‘training’ to increase alcohol tolerance is a risky phenomenon in a non-trivial number of college students. Notably, the majority of the sample (95.4%) was under the US legal drinking age (i.e. 21 years), suggesting ‘training’ occurred before students could legally drink. When young adults deliberately induce tolerance, they may increase the risk of alcohol poisoning and may prompt associated symptoms of alcohol dependence [12]. Importantly, these findings are potentially applicable to young adult populations in many countries where heavy drinking is problematic [13] and real or imagined social status is afforded to those who can ‘hold their liquor’.

‘Training’ challenges professional conceptualizations of the desirability of alcohol-related pathology, although it should not be surprising that many youths would want to drink at a party without feeling too intoxicated, sedated or sick. Nevertheless, some of these individuals, rather than moderating drinking to avoid negative consequences (and reduce risk), opt to enhance tolerance and, presumably, increase their likelihood of becoming alcohol-dependent.

Notable limitations of this work can inform future research directions. For example, no ‘training’ questionnaire existed previously, and it could certainly have been more exhaustive (e.g. including questions about drinking games, ‘training’ goals and additional alcohol measures and correlates) and more rigorous (e.g. counterbalancing questions to reduce respondent biases). It did not delineate fully whether ‘training’ is employed for specific events, or for becoming a ‘better’ heavy drinker in general. Furthermore, longitudinal research could capture more effectively ‘training’ processes, drinking behaviors, tolerance increases over time, the durability of induced tolerance and could address the alternative hypothesis that ‘training’ reflects incidental increases in tolerance (and exists merely as a way to rationalize increased tolerance as an intended result). Nationally representative and cross-national studies could help to determine the full scope of the problem, also elucidating whether ‘training’ may be a function of minimum legal drinking age laws.

This phenomenon has clear implications for alcohol intervention efforts. That is, both prevention and treatment interventions might be developed to reduce the appeal of ‘training’, or the perceived importance of ‘badges of honor’ associated with heavy drinking. Similarly, cognitively reframing such ‘badges’ as ‘symptoms’ of dependence might help drinkers to view the dark side of increased tolerance. Educational and feedback-based interventions might incorporate some discussion on this possible paradox for students, that increased tolerance seems beneficial (by presumably allowing more drinking with less sickness) although is indeed quite harmful.

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by grants to Kenneth J. Sher, PhD (K05AA017242 and T32AA013526), Douglas Steinley PhD (K25 AA017456), Julia A. Martinez (F31 AA018590) and Andrew C. Heath PhD (P50 AA11998), from the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism. All the authors have full access to all the data in the study and take responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis. The authors would like to thank Margie Gurwit BA (University of Missouri) for her assistance in data management. The authors would also like to thank Molly Trull (University of Missouri) for her assistance in data collection.

Footnotes

Declarations of interest

None.

References

1. American Psychiatric Association . Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 4th. American Psychiatric Association; Washington, DC: 2000. text revision. [Google Scholar]

2. Saha TD, Stinson FS, Grant BF. The role of alcohol consumption in future classifications of alcohol use disorders. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2007;89:82–92. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

3. Schuckit MA, Smith TL, Hesselbrock V, Bucholz KK, Beirut L, Edenberg H, et al. Clinical implications of tolerance to alcohol in nondependent young drinkers. Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse. 2008;34:133–49. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

4. Johnston LD, O’Malley PM, Bachman JG, Schulenberg JE. Monitoring the Future National Survey Results on Drug Use, 1975–2008, vol. II: College Students and Adults Ages 19–50. National Institute on Drug Abuse; Bethesda, MD: 2009. (NIH Publication no. 09-7403) [Google Scholar]

5. Hingson R, Heeren T, Winter M, Wechsler H. Magnitude of alcohol-related mortality and morbidity among U.S. college students ages 18–24: changes from 1998 to 2001. Annu Rev Pub Health. 2005;26:259–79. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

6. Mallett KA, Lee CM, Turrisi R, Larimer ME. Assessing college students’ perceptions of tolerance to alcohol using social cues: the social tolerance index. Addict Behav. 2009;34:219–22. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

7. Chandrasekhar J. Beerfest. United States; Warner Bros. Pictures: 2006. (Director) [Motion Picture] [Google Scholar]

8. University Registrar Fall 2008 enrollment summary. 2010 Available at: http://registrar.missouri.edu/statistics/fall-2008/index.php (accessed 3 February 2010)

9. Wechsler H, Dowdall GW, Davenport A, Castillo S. Correlates of college student binge drinking. Am J Public Health. 1995;85:921–6. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

10. Rutledge P, Park A, Sher KJ. 21st birthday drinking: extremely extreme. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2008;76:511–6. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

11. Grekin E, Sher KJ, Krull J. College spring break and alcohol use: effects of selection and spring break activity. J Stud Alcohol Drugs. 2007;68:681–8. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

12. Sher KJ, Wolf ST, Martinez JA. How can etiological research inform the distinction between normal drinking and disordered drinking? In: Scheier LM, editor. Handbook of Drug Use Etiology. American Psychological Association; Washington, DC: 2009. pp. 225–45. [Google Scholar]

13. World Health Organization . Global Status Report: Alcohol Policy. World Health Organization; Geneva, Switzerland: 2004. [Google Scholar]

Deliberate induction of alcohol tolerance: empirical introduction to a novel health risk (2024)
Top Articles
Latest Posts
Article information

Author: Lilliana Bartoletti

Last Updated:

Views: 6133

Rating: 4.2 / 5 (53 voted)

Reviews: 84% of readers found this page helpful

Author information

Name: Lilliana Bartoletti

Birthday: 1999-11-18

Address: 58866 Tricia Spurs, North Melvinberg, HI 91346-3774

Phone: +50616620367928

Job: Real-Estate Liaison

Hobby: Graffiti, Astronomy, Handball, Magic, Origami, Fashion, Foreign language learning

Introduction: My name is Lilliana Bartoletti, I am a adventurous, pleasant, shiny, beautiful, handsome, zealous, tasty person who loves writing and wants to share my knowledge and understanding with you.