Behavioral and Neural Signatures of Visual Imagery Vividness Extremes: Aphantasia versus Hyperphantasia (2024)

Article Navigation

Article Contents

  • Abstract

  • Introduction

  • Materials and Methods

  • Results

  • Discussion

  • Notes

  • Funding

  • Author Contributions

  • References

Journal Article

,

Fraser Milton

Discipline of Psychology, University of Exeter

, Exeter EX4 4QG,

UK

Search for other works by this author on:

,

Jon Fulford

Cognitive Neurology Research Group, University of Exeter Medical School, College House

, Exeter EX1 2LU,

UK

Search for other works by this author on:

,

Carla Dance

Cognitive Neurology Research Group, University of Exeter Medical School, College House

, Exeter EX1 2LU,

UK

Search for other works by this author on:

,

James Gaddum

Cognitive Neurology Research Group, University of Exeter Medical School, College House

, Exeter EX1 2LU,

UK

Search for other works by this author on:

,

Brittany Heuerman-Williamson

Cognitive Neurology Research Group, University of Exeter Medical School, College House

, Exeter EX1 2LU,

UK

Search for other works by this author on:

,

Kealan Jones

Cognitive Neurology Research Group, University of Exeter Medical School, College House

, Exeter EX1 2LU,

UK

Search for other works by this author on:

,

Kathryn F Knight

Discipline of Psychology, University of Exeter

, Exeter EX4 4QG,

UK

Search for other works by this author on:

,

Matthew MacKisack

Cognitive Neurology Research Group, University of Exeter Medical School, College House

, Exeter EX1 2LU,

UK

Search for other works by this author on:

,

Crawford Winlove

Cognitive Neurology Research Group, University of Exeter Medical School, College House

, Exeter EX1 2LU,

UK

Search for other works by this author on:

Adam Zeman

Cognitive Neurology Research Group, University of Exeter Medical School, College House

, Exeter EX1 2LU,

UK

Address correspondence to Adam Zeman. Email: a.zeman@exeter.ac.uk

Search for other works by this author on:

Fraser Milton, Jon Fulford and Adam Zeman contributed equally to this work

Author Notes

Cerebral Cortex Communications, Volume 2, Issue 2, 2021, tgab035, https://doi.org/10.1093/texcom/tgab035

Published:

05 May 2021

Article history

Received:

16 January 2021

Revision received:

18 April 2021

Accepted:

20 April 2021

Published:

05 May 2021

  • Behavioral and Neural Signatures of Visual Imagery Vividness Extremes: Aphantasia versus Hyperphantasia (2)PDF
  • Split View
  • Views
    • Article contents
    • Figures & tables
    • Video
    • Audio
    • Supplementary Data
  • Cite

    Cite

    Fraser Milton, Jon Fulford, Carla Dance, James Gaddum, Brittany Heuerman-Williamson, Kealan Jones, Kathryn F Knight, Matthew MacKisack, Crawford Winlove, Adam Zeman, Behavioral and Neural Signatures of Visual Imagery Vividness Extremes: Aphantasia versus Hyperphantasia, Cerebral Cortex Communications, Volume 2, Issue 2, 2021, tgab035, https://doi.org/10.1093/texcom/tgab035

    Close

Search

Close

Search

Search Menu

Abstract

Although Galton recognized in the 1880s that some individuals lack visual imagery, this phenomenon was mostly neglected over the following century. We recently coined the terms “aphantasia” and “hyperphantasia” to describe visual imagery vividness extremes, unlocking a sustained surge of public interest. Aphantasia is associated with subjective impairment of face recognition and autobiographical memory. Here we report the first systematic, wide-ranging neuropsychological and brain imaging study of people with aphantasia (n = 24), hyperphantasia (n = 25), and midrange imagery vividness (n = 20). Despite equivalent performance on standard memory tests, marked group differences were measured in autobiographical memory and imagination, participants with hyperphantasia outperforming controls who outperformed participants with aphantasia. Face recognition difficulties and autistic spectrum traits were reported more commonly in aphantasia. The Revised NEO Personality Inventory highlighted reduced extraversion in the aphantasia group and increased openness in the hyperphantasia group. Resting state fMRI revealed stronger connectivity between prefrontal cortices and the visual network among hyperphantasic than aphantasic participants. In an active fMRI paradigm, there was greater anterior parietal activation among hyperphantasic and control than aphantasic participants when comparing visualization of famous faces and places with perception. These behavioral and neural signatures of visual imagery vividness extremes validate and illuminate this significant but neglected dimension of individual difference.

aphantasia, autobiographical, hyperphantasia, imagery, neuroimaging

Introduction

Vision is our preeminent sense modality, deployed in the here and now. Most of us can also imagine the appearances of scenes and objects in their absence, enjoying an experience that is typically less vivid than “real seeing” but nonetheless has a distinctively visual feel: We refer to this as the experience of visual imagery (Pearson 2019). Visual imagery plays a prominent role, for most of us, in the subjective experience of daydreams, autobiographical recollection, future thinking, and nocturnal dreaming (Schacter and Addis 2007; Smallwood and Schooler 2015); it can influence perception (Pearson 2019), exerts a potent influence on emotions (Holmes and Mathews 2010), and has implicated in creativity and mental practice (Shepard 1988; Munzert and Lorey 2013). Yet the experience of imagery proves to be highly variable. We have recently described individuals with a lifelong lack of visual imagery, or “aphantasia,” and others whose imagery is “as vivid as real seeing” or “hyperphantasia” (Zeman etal. 2015; Zeman etal. 2020). Here, we report the first systematic study of the neuropsychological and neural signatures of aphantasia and hyperphantasia.

The 19th-century psychologist, Francis Galton, devised the earliest measure of the vividness of visual imagery (Galton 1880). Using his “breakfast table questionnaire” that invited participants to rate the “illumination, definition and coloring” of “your breakfast table as you sat down to it this morning,” Galton recognized that in some participants “the powers [of visualization] are zero” (p. 306). However, this phenomenon, the lifelong absence of a mind’s eye, has been almost entirely neglected until our recent descriptions (Zeman etal. 2015; Zeman etal. 2020). The only exception is a study suggesting that 2–3% of the undergraduate population are “lifelong non-imagers” (Faw 2009).

Previous work focusing on less extreme variations in imagery vividness indicates that they exert a particularly strong influence on autobiographical recollection (Palombo etal. 2018): Imagery vividness correlates positively with the fluency of autobiographical recall and the density of sensory details in autobiographical recollection (D'Argembeau and Van der Linden 2006; Vannucci etal. 2016). More vivid autobiographical memories, in turn, are associated with stronger activation in visual cortices at the time of recollection (Gilboa etal. 2004) and stronger resting state connectivity between the medial temporal and occipital lobes (Sheldon etal. 2016). Disorders affecting vision itself and visual imagery have been linked to autobiographical memory impairment (Rubin and Greenberg 1998; Greenberg and Knowlton 2014; Ahmed etal. 2018).

Beyond the domain of memory, studies focusing on the vividness of “object imagery” suggest that high—but not extreme—trait vividness scores are associated with enhanced performance on certain perceptual tasks, such as identifying degraded figures, and are overrepresented among artists; high “spatial imagery” scores, in contrast, are associated with enhanced performance in visuospatial tasks and are overrepresented among scientists (Blazhenkova and Kozhevnikov 2010). There is evidence for an association between lifelong face recognition difficulty—congenital prosopagnosia—and reduced imagery vividness (Gruter etal. 2009), and, conversely, an association between synesthesia and elevated levels of imagery vividness (Barnett and Newell 2008).

The neural basis of typical visual imagery has been explored extensively in recent decades, building on extensive preceding reflection and research on the cognitive nature of imagery (Kosslyn etal. 2006; Mackisack etal. 2016). The “visualization network” involves a combination of regions, including frontoparietal regions subserving executive/attentional control and the control of eye movements; areas involved in memory, with overlap with the default mode network; and “visual” regions including the fusiform and primary visual cortices (Winlove etal. 2018; Pearson 2019). The process of visualization has been conceptualized as “vision in reverse”: During imagery, the usual flow of information from the retina through the visual system to multimodal cortices involved in the control of behavior is reversed, exploiting the extensive feedback pathways that ordinarily play a more subtle and unobtrusive role in vision (Friston etal. 2012; Pearson 2019; Breedlove etal. 2020). The neural correlates of imagery vividness, specifically, are controversial: There is evidence for an inverse relationship between the strength and vividness of visual imagery and the surface area and excitability of the primary visual cortex, in contrast to positive relationships with the surface area of frontal regions (Bergmann etal. 2016; Keogh etal. 2020). Vividness correlates with the degree of activation of higher-order visual areas and regions of the limbic system or default mode network (Fulford etal. 2018). The neural basis of extreme imagery vividness has not been investigated previously, with the exception of case studies of patients whose imagery vividness has been perturbed by neurological or psychiatric disorder (Farah 1984; Bartolomeo 2008; Zeman etal. 2010).

Our description of “aphantasia” in 2015 attracted global publicity, resulting in email contact from >14 000 individuals, the majority describing lifelong aphantasia, but many also reporting its converse, lifelong hyperphantasia (Zeman etal. 2020). Data from a community-based biobank suggest a prevalence of 0.7% for “extreme aphantasia” and 2.5% for “extreme hyperphantasia,” defined as floor and ceiling scores on the Vividness of Visual Imagery Questionnaire (VVIQ) (Zeman etal. 2020). A questionnaire-based study among individuals with extreme imagery who contacted us spontaneously indicates that aphantasia is associated with reported difficulty in autobiographical memory and face recognition, while hyperphantasia is associated with an increased frequency of synesthesia (Zeman etal. 2020). Participants with aphantasia were significantly more likely to work in scientific or mathematical domains, while participants with hyperphantasia were more often employed in “creative” professions. Visual aphantasia is variably associated with the absence of imagery in other modalities, while visual imagery during dreaming is often preserved, suggesting a dissociation between wakeful and dreaming imagery. The frequency of aphantasia in first-degree relatives is elevated, hinting at a possible genetic contribution to imagery vividness. Although we did not enquire systematically about the presence of autistic spectrum disorder in our participants with extreme imagery, many people with aphantasia mentioned spontaneously that they fall on the autistic spectrum. Dawes etal. (2020) have recently reported broadly similar results from an independent questionnaire survey.

While these findings are suggestive, they rely on first-person report: Introspection can, of course, be fallible (Zeman etal. 2020). Differences in visual imagery vividness judgments could reflect differences in metacognition rather than true differences in vividness. There is therefore a need to triangulate these first-person findings with more objective measurements of both behavioral and neural correlates of visual imagery extremes. Such evidence has begun to accumulate in recent reports: People with aphantasia are unable to use imagery to modulate perception during binocular rivalry (Keogh and Pearson 2018); exhibit a reduced galvanic skin response to visually evocative narratives with a strong emotional impact on control participants (Wicken etal. 2019); show reduced precision of visual working memory (Jacobs etal. 2018); and recall fewer objects and colors from studied scenes than control participants (Bainbridge etal. 2021). These findings suggest that people reporting aphantasia have introspected a distinctive trait with measurable implications for psychological functioning.

We recruited participants with aphantasia, hyperphantasia, and average imagery vividness to investigate a range of hypotheses based on our questionnaire data. On the behavioral plane, we predicted that individuals with aphantasia would display impairments of autobiographical memory, future and atemporal imagination, and face recognition. On the neural plane, we predicted that there would be brain imaging signatures of visual imagery vividness extremes, which we investigated using a multimodal approach (structural MRI, task-based fMRI, resting state fMRI [rsfMRI], and diffusion tensor imaging). We suspected, more tentatively, that there might be differences in personality or traits relevant to autistic spectrum disorder between people with aphantasia and hyperphantasia. This suspicion is supported by some recent evidence (Dance etal. 2021). Finally, our previous work (Zeman etal. 2010) suggested that tests traditionally thought to be sensitive to differences in imagery vividness might discriminate poorly between these groups.

We note that the term “imagination” is ambiguous, as it can refer, among other things, to the ability to summon up an image of a specific item (“imagine an apple”) and to the richer ability to represent some possible or future scene or event (“imagine the next party you will go to”). We distinguish these two senses here by using the term “visualization” to refer to the former and “imagination” to refer to the latter.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Sixty-nine participants completed the study. Twenty-four participants had aphantasia, defined as having a Vividness of Visual Imagery Questionnaire (modified VVIQ; Marks 1973) score between 16–23/80 (Zeman etal. 2020), and twenty-five had hyperphantasia, defined as a VVIQ score between 75 and 80 (Zeman etal. 2020). Participants with aphantasia and hyperphantasia were recruited from a variety of sources (spontaneous email contacts to the research team, the Extend Biobank, and members of the University community). They reported that their imagery vividness had been a lifelong trait (i.e., not acquired). Twenty control participants with VVIQ scores in the midrange (55–60) were recruited from a local Biobank (http://exeter.crf.nihr.ac.uk/extend). The mean score VVIQ among the control participants (56.95) was similar to the mean score in our previous community-based control group (58.6). All participants (with aphantasia, hyperphantasia, and controls) satisfied the requirements that 1) they could attend in person on two occasions for testing and scanning; 2) they were otherwise healthy; and 3) they were right-handed. Seven participants (three with aphantasia, three with hyperphantasia, and one control) underwent neuropsychological testing but not MRI scanning for a variety of reasons (obesity, metallic implants, scanner failure, and logistical reasons) and were therefore excluded from the study. Ethical approval for this study was obtained from the University of Exeter Psychology Ethics Committee. As expected, there was a significant overall difference in the VVIQ score between groups, F(2,66) = 5245.53, P < 0.001, η2p = 0.994. Pairwise comparisons indicated that the VVIQ score for the aphantasia group (M = 16.92, SD = 1.47) was significantly lower than for both the control (M = 56.95, SD = 2.93) and hyperphantasia (M = 77.08, SD = 1.75) groups (both Ps < 0.001), while the VVIQ score for the hyperphantasia group was significantly higher than for the control group (P < 0.001).

Behavioral Methods

Neuropsychological Profile

Standard neuropsychological tests were used to investigate general intelligence (Weschler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence; Wechsler 2011 or the Weschler Adult Intelligence Scale; Wechsler 2008) and executive functioning (Trails test and letter [FAS] and animal fluency, e.g., Steinberg etal. 2005). Depression and anxiety were assessed using the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (Zigmond and Snaith 1983). The Autism Spectrum Quotient questionnaire (Baron-Cohen etal. 2001) and the NEO Five-Factor Inventory-3 (McCrae and Costa 2010) were also administered.

Anterograde Memory

Anterograde memory was measured using the Logical Memory Test (immediate and 30min delayed recall as well as 30min delayed recognition of a prose passage) (Wechsler 1997), the Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure Test (copy and 30min delayed recall) (Osterreith and Rey 1944), and the Warrington Recognition Memory Test (Warrington 1984) for word and face recognition.

Visual Imagery Tests

We used the Manikins test (Ratcliff 1979) to assess mental rotation. In this test, a manikin holding a black disk in one hand was either presented facing the participant or with its back to the participant and was standing either the right way up or upside down. Participants had to say which hand the black disk was in. There were 3 practice trials and 32 experimental trials.

In the curved segment test (van der Meulen etal. 2009), each letter in the alphabet was shown in lower case. Participants were asked to say whether the upper-case letter (not shown) corresponding to each lower-case letter has a curved line segment to it (e.g., “A” would not have a curved segment, while “P” would).

In the animal tails test (Behrmann etal. 1994), the names of 40 animals were presented (e.g., hedgehog and squirrel) and participants were asked to judge whether the animal had a short or a long tail relative to its body size. In all three tests, accuracy was the dependent measure.

Future and Atemporal Imagination

The future and atemporal imagination tasks were closely based on the procedures previously described by Hassabis etal. (2007). For the atemporal imagination task, participants were provided with three scenarios (1) imagine that you are sitting on a white sandy beach on a beautiful tropical bay; 2) imagine that you are standing in the main hall of a museum containing many exhibits; 3) imagine that you are standing in the middle of a bustling street market), read out by the experimenter. Participants were asked to imagine each scenario and describe it in as much detail as possible. Participants were told not to recount an actual memory or any part of one but to create something new. Participants continued until they came to a natural end. Extra details were then sought by the experimenter via general probes (e.g., can you see anything else in the scene?). The experimenter did not introduce any piece of information that had not already been described by the participant. After this, the participants rated their scenarios on a scale of 1 to 5 in terms of their sense of presence (1, “did not feel like I was there at all”; 5, “felt strongly like I was really there”) and perceived salience (1, “couldn’t really see anything”; 5, “extremely salient”).

In the future imagination task, participants were asked to imagine three events in the near future (1) possible Christmas event; 2) possible event over next weekend; 3) possible future meeting with a friend) that were specific in time and place, and realistic in nature. Participants were told that it should not be a past event transferred to the future. As in the atemporal imagination test, participants were asked to imagine the event and describe it in as much detail as possible. After the participants had finished their description, the experimenter provided general prompts to extract further information. At the end of the scenario, participants were again asked to rate on a scale from 1 to 5 their sense of presence and perceived salience.

Scoring for both tasks was conducted in the manner described by Hassabis etal. (Hassabis etal. 2007) with the exception that we did not include the spatial coherence index measure. The reason for this is that this self-report index asks participants specifically to describe the nature of the visual imagery they are experiencing (e.g., “It was a collection of separate images.”), which would be difficult for the aphantasia group to rate in the same way as the other groups. This resulted in a composite score out of 54 comprising three components. For the first component—Content—the scorer (K.K.), who was blind to the groups to which participants belonged, classified statements by the participant as belonging to one of the following four categories: spatial reference, entity presence, sensory description, or thought/emotion/action. Repetitions or irrelevant information was not scored. Following the original protocol (Hassabis etal. 2007), the score for the number of unique details in a category was capped at 7, making a total content score of 28. The second component was the Participant ratings detailed above (although the ratings were rescaled from 1–5 to 0–4, the overall measure was scored out of 8). The final component was the scorer’s own assessment of the extent to which the description evoked a detailed picture of the experience in their own mind’s eye (0, no picture at all; 10, vivid, extremely rich picture). Following the original protocol (Hassabis etal. 2007), this score was multiplied by 1.8 to give a measure scored out of 18. We averaged the composite score of the three scenarios for both the atemporal and future imagination tasks (One control participant was excluded from the future imagination task due to technical problems with the audio recording. For similar reasons, the average future imagination score for one of the hyperphantasia participants was based on two rather than three scenarios.). A second scorer (C.D.) analyzed a subset of the descriptions (32%) independently. Coefficients showed that the agreement between the scorers was high for both the atemporal (0.99) and future imagination (0.97) tasks.

Autobiographical Memory

The Autobiographical Interview (Levine etal. 2002) involved three phases: A recall stage where participants were asked to recollect as much information as they could about a unique, temporally restricted, event that they were personally involved in for both an event in the most recent 5years (but not in the previous 3months) and an event that was more than 5years old (the remote time period); a general probe consisting of nonspecific questions (e.g., “can you provide more details?”); and a specific probe where participants underwent a semistructured interview in order to extract as much contextual detail as possible. The specific probe was administered after completing the free recall and general probe phases for each memory.

The memories were audiorecorded, transcribed, and scored using standard procedures (Levine etal. 2002). Specifically, narratives were segmented into details classified as “internal” or “external.” Internal details were episodic information specific to the selected event that can be subdivided into event, perceptual, time, place, and emotion/thought details. External details included information extrinsic to the event and consisted of semantic (factual information or extended events) and “other” (e.g., metacognitive statements, editorializing, and inferences) details. Contextual information that was not part of the chosen episode was also classified as external detail. Repetitions were scored but not included as either internal or external details (c.f., Milton etal. 2010). Additionally, qualitative ratings were assigned to each memory (Levine etal. 2002). The time, place, perceptual, and emotion/thought subcategories were rated on a scale from 0 (no information relating to that subcategory) to 3 (specific, rich detail pertaining to the subcategory). Episodic richness was scored on a 0–6 scale to account for its greater importance. A time integration measure (on a 0–3 scale) assessed the integration of the episode into a larger time scale. The ratings summed to 21. The interviews were all analyzed by one scorer (F.M.), for consistency, who was blind to the groups to which participants belonged. A second scorer (C.D.) analyzed a subset of the memories (32%) independently. Coefficients showed that the agreement between the scorers was high for the internal details (0.89), external details (0.92), and the qualitative ratings (0.95).

Face and Buildings Recognition

Participants completed the 20-item Prosopagnosia Index (PI20) questionnaire (Shah etal. 2015) to test for perceived face recognition difficulties. In addition, we developed a 15-item famous faces test, in which participants were presented with four similar faces and they had to pick out which was the famous face (e.g., Barack Obama and George Clooney). In the graded buildings test (Evans etal. 1995), participants were required to identify 30 buildings varying from easy (e.g., the Leaning Tower of Pisa) to hard (e.g., Sagrada Familia). Each building was scored out of 2 (1 mark for correct names, 1 for correct location).

Neuropsychological tests were administered in the same order to all participants during a single testing session, lasting approximately 3h. If participants became fatigued during testing, they were encouraged to take a break, but this was seldom required. Questionnaires were completed prior to the scanning session. For logistical reasons, neuroimaging and neuropsychological testing were performed on the same day in 30 participants, on different days in 39 participants.

Correction for Multiple Comparisons

As there is no consensus on the appropriate correction for multiple comparisons in neuropsychological studies, we have given uncorrected probabilities but also indicate whether these survive a strict Bonferroni correction on the basis that we have performed 34 tests (0.05/34 = 0.0015). To avoid repetition, we indicate the findings that remain significant at the corrected threshold with a bold asterisk*.

Neuroimaging Methods

Scanning was performed using a 1.5 T system (Intera, Philips, the Netherlands) at the Exeter University Magnetic Resonance Research Centre (Exeter, UK). fMRI was undertaken using a T2*-weighted single-shot echoplanar (EPI) scanning sequence (Repetition time [TR]= 3s, Echo time [TE]= 50ms, resolution 2.88 × 2.88 × 3.6mm, 38 slices, 90° flip angle) and comprised two imaging runs, each of 346 dynamics, resulting in a scanning time of 17.5min per run. Resting state data were acquired using a T2*-weighted single-shot EPI scanning sequence (TR = 3s, TE = 50ms, resolution 2.5 × 2.5 × 3.5mm, 37 slices, 90° flip angle) with 160 repetitions and a total acquisition time of 8min. Diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) (TR = 9470ms, TE = 66ms, resolution 2.3 × 2.3 × 3mm, 48 slices) was undertaken with 32 diffusion directions with b values of 0 and 1000smm−2. High-resolution T1-weighted anatomical images, which covered the whole head and were centered approximately over the anterior commissure, were acquired with a resolution of 0.9 × 0.9 × 0.9mm. The order of acquisition was as follows: survey scans, fMRI, resting state, structural, DTI.

Resting State fMRI

During the resting state protocol, participants were instructed to stay awake, keep their eyes open, and look at the projection screen on which was displayed a cross. They were further instructed to look in the general direction of the cross but not to fixate on it in a concentrated fashion. Resting state analysis was undertaken in the CONN software package (www.nitrc.org/projects/conn, RRID:SCR_009550). Initially, preprocessing was carried out on the raw data, consisting of realignment, normalization, outlier detection (ART-based scrubbing. Using the 95th percentile in normative data, a global signal z-value threshold of 3, and participant motion threshold of 0.5mm), and smoothing (8mm). Regions of interest (ROI) were then specified based on the FSL Harvard-Oxford atlas (Desikan etal. 2006) covering cortical and subcortical areas, the automated anatomical labeling atlas (AAL) (Tzourio-Mazoyer etal. 2002) for cerebellar regions, and the CONN list of commonly used networks (Whitfield-Gabrieli and Nieto-Castanon 2012). Denoising was subsequently undertaken to remove motion associated with physiological and artefactual effects via the use of linear regression (using global white matter signal, CSF signal, realignment parameters, and scrubbing parameters as confounds) and band-pass filtering. Based on previous findings (Winlove etal. 2018; Pearson 2019), analysis was then undertaken at a seed-to-voxel level using the following regions/networks as the initial seed: Frontoparietal lateral prefrontal cortex (LPFC) network, dorsal attention network, visual–occipital network, and the hippocampus. Significance was defined as occurring with an uncorrected P < 0.001 for the height threshold and P < 0.05 for the FDR corrected cluster size.

Task-Based fMRI

Prior to the scanning protocol, participants were shown a series of color images corresponding to famous places and faces and asked to identify them. From this larger set, a subset of 40 famous faces with neutral expressions presented from the front and 40 famous places, which the participants were familiar with and had correctly identified, were used for the subsequent fMRI protocol.

During each of two runs of the fMRI protocol, participants undertook a modified version of the task performed by MX (Zeman etal. 2010), which consisted of three different tasks, grouped into blocks and separated by a blank white screen displayed for between 0.5 and 2.5s, with the exact length randomized from set to set. The task was undertaken within a darkened scanner room where the only illumination was provided via the projection of the task images that filled the full visual field of the participants in the direction they were facing. In the first “Perception” block, a famous place or face image was presented for 6s, followed by a response screen for 2s where participants were instructed to rate the pleasantness of the image they saw on a 1–3 scale (from least to most pleasant) via a three-button response pad, activated by their right (dominant) hand, having previously ensured all text was clearly readable by the participant within the scanner. Subsequently, a blank screen was shown for a randomized length between 0.5 and 2.5s. Four sequential image/response/blank screen sets were presented per block. The following “Control” block (Stark and Squire 2001) consisted of a single-digit number shown for 2s followed by a feedback screen for 2s where participants were instructed to indicate whether the number was odd or even via the first two buttons of the three-button response pad. A blank screen was then shown for a randomized length between 0.5 and 2.5s. Four trials were presented per block. In the final “Imagery” block, participants were shown a screen for 6s instructing them to imagine a named person or place, corresponding to one of the images they had seen earlier in the protocol (the specific instruction was to “think about” the person or place). This was then followed by a feedback screen for 2s where participants were instructed to rate the vividness of the visualization experience on a 1–3 scale (least to most vivid) via the three-button response pad, after which a blank screen was shown for a randomized length between 0.5 and 2.5s. Four sequential image/feedback/blank screen sets were presented per block. The sequence of Perception–Control–Imagery blocks outlined above was repeated 10 times per imaging run, resulting in 40 cases of each stimuli type per run.

Feedback responses generated within the task in terms of visualization vividness and pleasantness of images were analyzed by one-way ANOVAs to examine differences between groups and by paired t-tests to assess differences within groups between the “face” and “place” tasks. All subsequent data analysis was undertaken using SPM12 software (www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm). The data from the two separate fMRI runs were treated as separate sessions within the analysis, which consisted of images being slice time corrected, realigned, coregistered to the T1 structural images, normalized to the Montreal Neurological Institute template (MNI305), and smoothed using a Gaussian kernel of 8mm full-width half-maximum. Following estimation using a general linear model, employing a hemodynamic response function, together with temporal and dispersion derivatives to model the blood oxygen level–dependent response and including six head movement parameters as regressors, statistical analysis was carried out to compare activation patterns associated with the Perception, Control, and Imagery conditions for each individual. Whole brain comparisons were then undertaken at a groupwise level with a combined statistical threshold of P <0.001 and a threshold of 28 contiguous voxels (voxel size: 3 × 3 × 3mm), which together produce an overall corrected threshold of P < 0.05. These values were estimated using AlphaSim as implemented in the REST toolbox (Version 1.8, Song etal. 2011). A smoothing estimate of 9.3, 9.1, 9.2mm was used (this was a group-level estimate calculated in SPM12 using the group residuals from the general linear model, e.g., Kiebel etal. 1999).

Volumetric MRI

Differences between groups in sizes of brain substructures were assessed by undertaking voxel-based morphometry using the Dartel toolbox (Ashburner 2007) within SPM12 (the Wellcome Department of Cognitive Neurology, University College London). Data was segmented from individuals to generate gray and white matter images. Non-linear deformations of these images were established to ensure that these matched across all participants. Finally, data was normalised to MNI space. Statistical analysis was then undertaken via the use of a linear general model to examine systematic differences between the groups taking into account differences in total brain size between individuals. Significant changes were accepted at a P < 0.05 following family-wise error correction.

Diffusion Tensor Imaging

Voxelwise statistical analysis of the fractional anisotropy (FA) data was carried out using tract-based spatial statistics (TBSS, Smith etal. 2006), part of FSL (Smith etal. 2004). First, FA images were created by fitting a tensor model to the raw diffusion data using FDT, within the FSL toolbox, and then brain-extracted using BET (Smith 2002). All subjects’ FA data were then aligned into a common space using the nonlinear registration tool FNIRT (Andersson etal. 2007a, 2007b), which uses a b-spline representation of the registration warp field (Rueckert etal. 1999). A mean FA image was then created and thinned to create a mean FA skeleton that represents the centers of all tracts common to the group. Each subject’s aligned FA data were then projected onto this skeleton and the resulting data fed into voxelwise cross-subject statistics. Statistics were undertaken using the “Randomize” tool within FSL, employing the threshold-free cluster enhancement (TFCE) method, which generates cluster-based thresholding corrected for multiple comparisons, with significant changes accepted at a P < 0.05.

Data-Sharing

Requests for data-sharing should be addressed to the authors and will be considered individually. We are keen to share data where possible, respecting the nature of the consent provided by our participants.

Results

Demographic and Behavioral Measures

The demographic, neuropsychological, and personality profiles of the aphantasia, control, and hyperphantasia groups are displayed in Table 1. There was no significant difference in age between the groups, F(2,66) = 0.12, P = 0.884, η2p = 0.004. There was also no difference in gender distribution across groups, χ2 (2,69) = 3.98, P = 0.136. However, IQ did differ significantly across the groups, F(2,66) = 5.18, P = 0.008, η2p = 0.136. Pairwise comparisons indicated that the aphantasia group had a significantly elevated IQ compared with the hyperphantasia group (P = 0.002), but there were no other statistically significant differences.

Table 1

Demographic, neuropsychological, and personality profile of the aphantasia, control, and hyperphantasia groups

Aphantasia mean (SD)Control mean (SD)Hyperphantasia mean (SD)
Gender14 male/10 female11 male/9 female8 male/17 female
Age, years33.71 (11.25)34.6 (12.78)35.36 (11.10)
IQ115.46 (12.54)110.00 (11.41)106.32 (4.51)
Trail A (s)22.84 (9.01)21.62 (5.79)20.42 (6.01)
Trail B (s)40.18 (9.45)40.74 (9.65)46.67 (16.74)
Letter fluency (words/min)15.65 (4.80)17.22 (3.35)16.60 (4.60)
Animal fluency (words/min)26.67 (5.70)28.20 (5.59)25.76 (5.43)
Autism quotient score (/50)22.04 (7.99)14.20 (6.24)17.92 (10.40)
Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale scores (max score)
 Anxiety (/21)7.00 (4.67)7.10 (2.83)7.16 (5.01)
 Depression (/21)4.21 (2.98)3.55 (3.10)2.28 (2.82)
Revised NEO PI (/25–75)
 Neuroticism48.96 (14.94)51.45 (10.30)48.96 (12.30)
 Agreeableness53.58 (12.03)54.65 (11.45)52.00 (10.47)
 Extraversion46.54 (11.62)57.75 (11.80)57.16 (13.82)
 Openness to experience53.21 (12.37)57.05 (10.40)65.16 (11.39)
 Conscientiousness42.13 (10.17)49.5 (9.65)47.96 (11.67)
Aphantasia mean (SD)Control mean (SD)Hyperphantasia mean (SD)
Gender14 male/10 female11 male/9 female8 male/17 female
Age, years33.71 (11.25)34.6 (12.78)35.36 (11.10)
IQ115.46 (12.54)110.00 (11.41)106.32 (4.51)
Trail A (s)22.84 (9.01)21.62 (5.79)20.42 (6.01)
Trail B (s)40.18 (9.45)40.74 (9.65)46.67 (16.74)
Letter fluency (words/min)15.65 (4.80)17.22 (3.35)16.60 (4.60)
Animal fluency (words/min)26.67 (5.70)28.20 (5.59)25.76 (5.43)
Autism quotient score (/50)22.04 (7.99)14.20 (6.24)17.92 (10.40)
Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale scores (max score)
 Anxiety (/21)7.00 (4.67)7.10 (2.83)7.16 (5.01)
 Depression (/21)4.21 (2.98)3.55 (3.10)2.28 (2.82)
Revised NEO PI (/25–75)
 Neuroticism48.96 (14.94)51.45 (10.30)48.96 (12.30)
 Agreeableness53.58 (12.03)54.65 (11.45)52.00 (10.47)
 Extraversion46.54 (11.62)57.75 (11.80)57.16 (13.82)
 Openness to experience53.21 (12.37)57.05 (10.40)65.16 (11.39)
 Conscientiousness42.13 (10.17)49.5 (9.65)47.96 (11.67)

Table 1

Demographic, neuropsychological, and personality profile of the aphantasia, control, and hyperphantasia groups

Aphantasia mean (SD)Control mean (SD)Hyperphantasia mean (SD)
Gender14 male/10 female11 male/9 female8 male/17 female
Age, years33.71 (11.25)34.6 (12.78)35.36 (11.10)
IQ115.46 (12.54)110.00 (11.41)106.32 (4.51)
Trail A (s)22.84 (9.01)21.62 (5.79)20.42 (6.01)
Trail B (s)40.18 (9.45)40.74 (9.65)46.67 (16.74)
Letter fluency (words/min)15.65 (4.80)17.22 (3.35)16.60 (4.60)
Animal fluency (words/min)26.67 (5.70)28.20 (5.59)25.76 (5.43)
Autism quotient score (/50)22.04 (7.99)14.20 (6.24)17.92 (10.40)
Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale scores (max score)
 Anxiety (/21)7.00 (4.67)7.10 (2.83)7.16 (5.01)
 Depression (/21)4.21 (2.98)3.55 (3.10)2.28 (2.82)
Revised NEO PI (/25–75)
 Neuroticism48.96 (14.94)51.45 (10.30)48.96 (12.30)
 Agreeableness53.58 (12.03)54.65 (11.45)52.00 (10.47)
 Extraversion46.54 (11.62)57.75 (11.80)57.16 (13.82)
 Openness to experience53.21 (12.37)57.05 (10.40)65.16 (11.39)
 Conscientiousness42.13 (10.17)49.5 (9.65)47.96 (11.67)
Aphantasia mean (SD)Control mean (SD)Hyperphantasia mean (SD)
Gender14 male/10 female11 male/9 female8 male/17 female
Age, years33.71 (11.25)34.6 (12.78)35.36 (11.10)
IQ115.46 (12.54)110.00 (11.41)106.32 (4.51)
Trail A (s)22.84 (9.01)21.62 (5.79)20.42 (6.01)
Trail B (s)40.18 (9.45)40.74 (9.65)46.67 (16.74)
Letter fluency (words/min)15.65 (4.80)17.22 (3.35)16.60 (4.60)
Animal fluency (words/min)26.67 (5.70)28.20 (5.59)25.76 (5.43)
Autism quotient score (/50)22.04 (7.99)14.20 (6.24)17.92 (10.40)
Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale scores (max score)
 Anxiety (/21)7.00 (4.67)7.10 (2.83)7.16 (5.01)
 Depression (/21)4.21 (2.98)3.55 (3.10)2.28 (2.82)
Revised NEO PI (/25–75)
 Neuroticism48.96 (14.94)51.45 (10.30)48.96 (12.30)
 Agreeableness53.58 (12.03)54.65 (11.45)52.00 (10.47)
 Extraversion46.54 (11.62)57.75 (11.80)57.16 (13.82)
 Openness to experience53.21 (12.37)57.05 (10.40)65.16 (11.39)
 Conscientiousness42.13 (10.17)49.5 (9.65)47.96 (11.67)

There was no difference between groups in either Trails A, F(2,66) = 0.70, P = 0.499, η2p = 0.02, or Trails B, F(2,66) = 1.95, P = 0.151, η2p = 0.06. There were also no differences between groups for letter (FAS), F(2,66) = 0.73, P = 0.486, η2p = 0.02, and Animals, F(2,66) = 1.07, P = 0.347, η2p = 0.03, verbal fluency.

On the HADS anxiety measure, there was no overall difference between groups, F(2,66) = 0.01, P = 0.992, η2p < 0.001. For depression, there was also no overall difference, F(2,66) = 2.69, P = 0.076, η2p = 0.08.

For the Autism Quotient measure, there was a significant overall difference between groups, F(2,66) = 4.64, P = 0.013, η2p = 0.12. Pairwise comparisons revealed that the aphantasia group had a significantly higher score than the controls (P = 0.003). There was no statistical difference in scores between the hyperphantasia and control groups (P = 0.151).

Turning to the revised NEO Personality Inventory, there was no overall difference between groups for Neuroticism, F(2,66) = 0.27, P = 0.764, η2p < 0.01, or Agreeableness, F(2,66) = 0.32, P = 0.731, η2p < 0.01 (in both cases, all pairwise comparisons, P > 0.4). However, there was a significant overall difference in terms of Extraversion, F(2,66) = 5.93, P = 0.004, η2p = 0.15. Pairwise comparisons revealed that the aphantasia group scored significantly lower on this measure than both the control (P = 0.004) and hyperphantasia (P = 0.004) groups. There was no difference in Extraversion between the control and hyperphantasia groups (P = 0.876). There were also differences between groups on the Openness to Experience measure, F(2,66) = 6.92, P = 0.002, η2p = 0.17, with the hyperphantasia group scoring significantly higher on this measure than both the aphantasia (P < 0.001) and control (P = 0.021) groups. There was no difference between the aphantasia and control groups (P = 0.273).

Anterograde Memory

The mean scores across groups for all the anterograde memory tests are shown in Table 2. There were no significant differences between groups for either the copy, F(2,66) = 0.82, P = 0.443, η2p = 0.02, or delayed recall, F(2,66) = 0.12, P = 0.294, η2p = 0.04, of the Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure Test.

Table 2

Performance on the anterograde memory tests across groups

Anterograde memory scores (max score)Aphantasia mean (SD)Control mean (SD)Hyperphantasia mean (SD)
Logical memory test
 Immediate recall (25)11.08 (3.44)13.45 (4.70)13.80 (4.29)
 Recall delay (25)10.58 (3.60)12.55 (4.86)12.16 (3.37)
 Recognition delay (15)12.21 (1.74)12.45 (2.01)13.00 (1.29)
Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure
 Copy (36)35.33 (1.09)35.70 (.92)35.64 (1.08)
 Delayed recall (36)23.17 (5.28)24.95 (3.99)25.08 (4.51)
Warrington Recognition Test
 Words (50)49.04 (1.40)49.10 (1.17)49.08 (1.08)
 Faces (50)42.79 (4.46)43.35 (4.85)44.48 (2.58)
Anterograde memory scores (max score)Aphantasia mean (SD)Control mean (SD)Hyperphantasia mean (SD)
Logical memory test
 Immediate recall (25)11.08 (3.44)13.45 (4.70)13.80 (4.29)
 Recall delay (25)10.58 (3.60)12.55 (4.86)12.16 (3.37)
 Recognition delay (15)12.21 (1.74)12.45 (2.01)13.00 (1.29)
Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure
 Copy (36)35.33 (1.09)35.70 (.92)35.64 (1.08)
 Delayed recall (36)23.17 (5.28)24.95 (3.99)25.08 (4.51)
Warrington Recognition Test
 Words (50)49.04 (1.40)49.10 (1.17)49.08 (1.08)
 Faces (50)42.79 (4.46)43.35 (4.85)44.48 (2.58)

Table 2

Performance on the anterograde memory tests across groups

Anterograde memory scores (max score)Aphantasia mean (SD)Control mean (SD)Hyperphantasia mean (SD)
Logical memory test
 Immediate recall (25)11.08 (3.44)13.45 (4.70)13.80 (4.29)
 Recall delay (25)10.58 (3.60)12.55 (4.86)12.16 (3.37)
 Recognition delay (15)12.21 (1.74)12.45 (2.01)13.00 (1.29)
Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure
 Copy (36)35.33 (1.09)35.70 (.92)35.64 (1.08)
 Delayed recall (36)23.17 (5.28)24.95 (3.99)25.08 (4.51)
Warrington Recognition Test
 Words (50)49.04 (1.40)49.10 (1.17)49.08 (1.08)
 Faces (50)42.79 (4.46)43.35 (4.85)44.48 (2.58)
Anterograde memory scores (max score)Aphantasia mean (SD)Control mean (SD)Hyperphantasia mean (SD)
Logical memory test
 Immediate recall (25)11.08 (3.44)13.45 (4.70)13.80 (4.29)
 Recall delay (25)10.58 (3.60)12.55 (4.86)12.16 (3.37)
 Recognition delay (15)12.21 (1.74)12.45 (2.01)13.00 (1.29)
Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure
 Copy (36)35.33 (1.09)35.70 (.92)35.64 (1.08)
 Delayed recall (36)23.17 (5.28)24.95 (3.99)25.08 (4.51)
Warrington Recognition Test
 Words (50)49.04 (1.40)49.10 (1.17)49.08 (1.08)
 Faces (50)42.79 (4.46)43.35 (4.85)44.48 (2.58)

For the immediate recall of the logical memory test, there was no statistically significant difference between groups, F(2,66) = 3.03, P = 0.055, η2p = 0.08. There were no overall differences between groups for either the 30min delayed recall, F(2,66) = 1.61, P = 0.209, η2p = 0.05, or recognition, F(2,66) = 1.42, P = 0.249, η2p = 0.04, tests. There were also no statistical differences between groups for the Warrington Recognition Words, F(2,66) = 0.01, P = 0.987, η2p < 0.001, and Faces, F(2,66) = 1.12, P = 0.332, η2p = 0.03, tests.

Visual Imagery Tests

The mean scores across groups for each of the visual imagery tests are displayed in Table 3. There was no overall difference between groups for the Manikins test, F(2,66) = 1.85, P = 0.165, η2p = 0.05, or for the curved segments test, F(2,66) = 0.24, P = 0.789, η2p = 0.01. For the animal tails test, the overall difference between conditions narrowly missed conventional statistical significance, F(2,66) = 3.07, P = 0.053, η2p = 0.09.

Table 3

Performance on the visual imagery tests across groups

Visual imagery test (max score)Aphantasia mean (SD)Control mean (SD)Hyperphantasia mean (SD)
Manikins test (32)30.42 (3.16)29.75 (2.47)31.20 (1.78)
Curved segments test (26)25.63 (0.92)25.65 (0.67)25.76 (0.52)
Animal tails test (40)32.92 (3.65)34.05 (2.74)35.00 (2.27)
Visual imagery test (max score)Aphantasia mean (SD)Control mean (SD)Hyperphantasia mean (SD)
Manikins test (32)30.42 (3.16)29.75 (2.47)31.20 (1.78)
Curved segments test (26)25.63 (0.92)25.65 (0.67)25.76 (0.52)
Animal tails test (40)32.92 (3.65)34.05 (2.74)35.00 (2.27)

Table 3

Performance on the visual imagery tests across groups

Visual imagery test (max score)Aphantasia mean (SD)Control mean (SD)Hyperphantasia mean (SD)
Manikins test (32)30.42 (3.16)29.75 (2.47)31.20 (1.78)
Curved segments test (26)25.63 (0.92)25.65 (0.67)25.76 (0.52)
Animal tails test (40)32.92 (3.65)34.05 (2.74)35.00 (2.27)
Visual imagery test (max score)Aphantasia mean (SD)Control mean (SD)Hyperphantasia mean (SD)
Manikins test (32)30.42 (3.16)29.75 (2.47)31.20 (1.78)
Curved segments test (26)25.63 (0.92)25.65 (0.67)25.76 (0.52)
Animal tails test (40)32.92 (3.65)34.05 (2.74)35.00 (2.27)

Future and Atemporal Imagination

The mean scores across conditions for the atemporal and future imagination tasks are displayed in Figure 1. For the atemporal imagination task, there was a significant overall difference between groups, F(2,66) = 86.88, P < 0.001*, η2p = 0.73, with the aphantasia group scoring significantly lower than the control and hyperphantasia groups (both Ps < 0.001), while the hyperphantasia group scored significantly higher than the control group (P = 0.001).

Figure 1

Mean composite score (maximum = 54) for each group in the atemporal and future imagination tasks. The thick line within the violin plots display the median score, the thinner lines reflect the 25th and 75th percentiles. The top and bottom of the violin plots illustrate the lowest and highest scores in the sample. The plots were created using Prism GraphPad Prism v. 9 (https://www.graphpad.com/scientific-software/prism/).

There was also a significant overall difference between groups for the future imagination task, F(2,65) = 41.21, P < 0.001*, η2p = 0.56, with pairwise comparisons revealing that the aphantasia group had a significantly lower composite score than the control and hyperphantasia groups (both Ps < 0.001). Furthermore, the hyperphantasia group had a significantly higher score than the control group (P = 0.005).

Whilst there was no clear departure from normality for any of the conditions, as a safety check we run the same analyses using the non-parametric Kruskal-Wallis test with follow-up pairwise comparisons conducted using Mann-Whitney U tests. These analyses led to exactly the same conclusions as the results reported above.

Autobiographical Memory

Mean scores for the internal and external details together with the qualitative ratings are shown in Figure 2. The mean number of internal details for the recent time period was analyzed using a one-way ANOVA and revealed a significant difference across groups, F(2,66) = 13.67, P < 0.001*, η2p = 0.293. Pairwise comparisons revealed that the aphantasia group produced significantly fewer internal details than both the control and hyperphantasia groups (both Ps < 0.01). In addition, the hyperphantasia group generated significantly more internal details than the control group (P = 0.041). There was also a significant effect for the remote time period, F(2,66) = 27.74, P < 0.001*, η2p = 0.457. Pairwise comparisons again showed that the aphantasia group recalled significantly fewer internal details than both the control and hyperphantasia groups (both Ps < 0.001), while the hyperphantasia group produced more internal details than the control group (P < 0.004).

Figure 2

Violin plots showing: (a) Mean number of internal details recalled for each time period; (b) mean rating (out of 21) for each time period; (c) mean number of external details recalled for each time period. The thick line within the violin plots displays the median score; the thinner lines reflect the 25th and 75th percentiles. The top and bottom of the violin plots illustrate the lowest and highest scores in the sample. The plots were created using Prism GraphPad Prism v. 9 (https://www.graphpad.com/scientific-software/prism/).

Analysis of the qualitative rating for the recent time period also yielded a significant overall difference between groups, F(2,66) = 7.19, P < 0.002, η2p = 0.30, with the aphantasia group having a significantly lower mean score than both the control (P = 0.018) and hyperphantasia (P < 0.001) groups. There was no statistical difference between the control and hyperphantasia groups (P = 0.273) although the hyperphantasia group had a numerically higher average. A similar pattern emerged for the remote time period—there was a significant overall effect, F(2,66) = 13.62, P < 0.001*, η2p = 0.292 with the aphantasia group having a lower score than the control (P = 0.002) and hyperphantasia groups (P < 0.001). There was no significant difference between the control and hyperphantasia groups (P = 0.119).

For external details, there was a significant overall difference between groups for the recent time period, F(2,66) = 3.51, P = 0.035, η2p = 0.096, with the hyperphantasia group producing significantly more details than the control group (P = 0.002). The other comparisons were not statistically different (Ps > 0.05). There was also a significant difference between groups for the remote time period, F(2,66) = 6.02, P = 0.004, η2p = 0.154. The hyperphantasia group recalled more external details than both the aphantasia (P = 0.044) and control (P = 0.001) groups. There was no statistical difference between the aphantasia and control group (P = 0.147).

For the autobiographical memory data, the data for some of the groups was of questionable normality. We again ran additional analyses using the Kruskal-Wallis test, using Mann-Whitney U tests for follow-up pairwise comparisons as a safety check. These analyses produced the identical pattern of results as presented above, with the exception that for the external details recent time period, the pairwise comparison revealed that the aphantasia group had a higher mean score than the control group (P = .040).

Finally, we calculated the ratio of internal:total details (c.f., Levine etal. 2002). These ratios for both remote and recent memories can be seen in Table 4. For the recent period, there was a significant overall difference between groups, F(2,69) = 18.97, P < 0.001*, η2p = 0.365. Pairwise comparisons revealed that the aphantasia group had a significantly lower ratio than both the control and hyperphantasia groups (both Ps < 0.001). There was no statistical difference between the hyperphantasia and control groups (P = 0.154). For the remote time period, there was also a significant overall difference between groups, F(2,66) = 23.50, P < 0.001*, η2p = 0.416. The aphantasia group again had a significantly lower ratio than the control and hyperphantasia groups (P < 0.001), but there was no statistically significant difference between the control and hyperphantasia groups (P = 0.095).

Table 4

Internal to total ratios for the recent and remote time periods

RecentRemote
MSDMSD
Aphantasia0.560.120.480.13
Hyperphantasia0.700.100.640.11
Control0.740.080.690.074
RecentRemote
MSDMSD
Aphantasia0.560.120.480.13
Hyperphantasia0.700.100.640.11
Control0.740.080.690.074

Table 4

Internal to total ratios for the recent and remote time periods

RecentRemote
MSDMSD
Aphantasia0.560.120.480.13
Hyperphantasia0.700.100.640.11
Control0.740.080.690.074
RecentRemote
MSDMSD
Aphantasia0.560.120.480.13
Hyperphantasia0.700.100.640.11
Control0.740.080.690.074

Face and Buildings Recognition

The mean scores for Prosopagnosia Index (PI) (Shah etal. 2015), the famous faces recognition test, and the graded buildings test are presented in Table 4. There was a significant overall difference between groups for the PI, F(2,66) = 12.83, P < 0.001*, η2p = 0.28, with the aphantasia group reporting their face recognition ability as significantly worse than both the control (P = 0.001) and hyperphantasia groups (P < 0.001). There was no difference in mean score between the control and hyperphantasia groups (P = 0.21).

In contrast to the results for the PI, there was no overall statistical difference between groups for the 15-item famous faces recognition test, F(2,66) = 0.67, P = 0.516, η2p = 0.02. The famous buildings test, though, did yield a significant overall difference between groups, F(2,66) = 3.43, P = 0.038, η2p = 0.09, with the aphantasia group performing significantly worse than the hyperphantasia group (P = 0.013). There were no other statistical differences (Ps > 0.5).

Brain Imaging Results

Resting State fMRI

rsfMRI revealed stronger connectivity between the visual–occipital network and several prefrontal regions (BAs 9, 10, 11) in the hyperphantasic group when compared with the aphantasic group (see Table 5 and Fig. 3). The left hippocampus was more strongly connected to the brain stem in the hyperphantasic group than the aphantasic group. Stronger resting state connectivity was detected in the aphantasic group than in the hyperphantasic group between the left hippocampus and a region of the anterior cingulate (left BA24); the left dorsal attention network and the middle frontal gyrus; the left frontoparietal control network and the left orbitofrontal cortex (left BA11). Stronger connectivity was also found for the aphantasic group than the control group between the left hippocampus and left BA10; the left dorsal attention network and the right BA8. No significant increase in connectivity was determined for the control group relative to either the aphantasic or hyperphantasic groups or for the hyperphantasic group relative to the control group for any of the regions examined.

Table 5

Mean scores on the prosopagnosia index questionnaire and the famous face recognition and graded buildings tests across groups

Aphantasia mean (SD)Control mean (SD)Hyperphantasia mean (SD)
Prosopagnosia Index (100)55.71 (13.23)43.30 (14.24)38.72 (8.480)
Famous face recognition (15)14.21 (1.22)14.40 (1.00)14.56 (9.96)
Graded buildings (60)44.54 (7.37)46.00 (7.46)49.72 (6.59)
Aphantasia mean (SD)Control mean (SD)Hyperphantasia mean (SD)
Prosopagnosia Index (100)55.71 (13.23)43.30 (14.24)38.72 (8.480)
Famous face recognition (15)14.21 (1.22)14.40 (1.00)14.56 (9.96)
Graded buildings (60)44.54 (7.37)46.00 (7.46)49.72 (6.59)

Table 5

Mean scores on the prosopagnosia index questionnaire and the famous face recognition and graded buildings tests across groups

Aphantasia mean (SD)Control mean (SD)Hyperphantasia mean (SD)
Prosopagnosia Index (100)55.71 (13.23)43.30 (14.24)38.72 (8.480)
Famous face recognition (15)14.21 (1.22)14.40 (1.00)14.56 (9.96)
Graded buildings (60)44.54 (7.37)46.00 (7.46)49.72 (6.59)
Aphantasia mean (SD)Control mean (SD)Hyperphantasia mean (SD)
Prosopagnosia Index (100)55.71 (13.23)43.30 (14.24)38.72 (8.480)
Famous face recognition (15)14.21 (1.22)14.40 (1.00)14.56 (9.96)
Graded buildings (60)44.54 (7.37)46.00 (7.46)49.72 (6.59)

Figure 3

Seed to voxel connectivity analysis using the visual–occipital network as initial seed displaying areas of greater connectivity for hyperphantasic than aphantasic participants.

Task-Based fMRI

In terms of participant feedback recorded during the fMRI task, there were significant differences (P < 0.001) between the groups in the vividness of the visualization experience (Mean ± SD: Aphantasic 1.08 ± 0.25, Control 2.39 ± 0.31, Hyperphantasic 2.81 ± 0.23). However, none of the groups reported a significant difference between the face and place tasks in terms of visualization vividness (P > 0.05). There was no significant difference (P > 0.05) between groups for reported pleasantness of presented images. However, there was a significantly higher pleasantness rating for “places” compared with “faces” (P < 0.001) for all three groups.

In terms of brain activation, there were no significant group differences when comparing perception and visualization with the control condition (see Supplementary Figs 15a,b). However, hyperphantasic participants activated a left anterior parietal region in the precentral gyrus (BAs 3 and 4), extending into BA 40, to a greater degree than aphantasic participants, in a subtraction of perception from visualization (see Table 6 and Fig. 4). A smaller, symmetrically placed area was more strongly activated in the right hemisphere. Two regions of the cerebellum, within the posterior and anterior lobes, and an area of the thalamus, were also more strongly activated in this comparison in the hyperphantasic group. In a similar comparison between aphantasic and control participants, the same left anterior parietal region was more strongly activated in the control sample. No difference was identified between the control and hyperphantasic groups in this comparison (Table 7).

Table 6

Resting state connectivity differences between aphantasic and hyperphantasic groups

NetworkComparisonMNI coordinates of center of clusterAnatomical region
Visual–occipitalHyper > Aphan−28, +56, +2Left BA10
+2, −92, −24Cerebellum
+40, +32, +38Right BA9
−24, +32, +36Left BA9
+8, +46, −12Right BA11
Hippocampus-leftHyper > Aphan−2, −24, −54Brain stem
Frontal parietal-leftAphan > Hyper−2, +56, −24Left BA11
Left dorsal attention IPS (intraparietal sulcus)Aphan > Hyper−32, +22, +38Middle frontal gyrus
Hippocampus-leftAphan > Hyper−10, −2, +40Left BA24
Left dorsal attention IPSAphan > Control+6, +36, +56Right BA8
Hippocampus-leftAphan > Control−42, +40, +22Left BA10
NetworkComparisonMNI coordinates of center of clusterAnatomical region
Visual–occipitalHyper > Aphan−28, +56, +2Left BA10
+2, −92, −24Cerebellum
+40, +32, +38Right BA9
−24, +32, +36Left BA9
+8, +46, −12Right BA11
Hippocampus-leftHyper > Aphan−2, −24, −54Brain stem
Frontal parietal-leftAphan > Hyper−2, +56, −24Left BA11
Left dorsal attention IPS (intraparietal sulcus)Aphan > Hyper−32, +22, +38Middle frontal gyrus
Hippocampus-leftAphan > Hyper−10, −2, +40Left BA24
Left dorsal attention IPSAphan > Control+6, +36, +56Right BA8
Hippocampus-leftAphan > Control−42, +40, +22Left BA10

Note: The table identifies networks more strongly connected in the hyperphantasic or aphantasic group and the targets of differential connectivity

Table 6

Resting state connectivity differences between aphantasic and hyperphantasic groups

NetworkComparisonMNI coordinates of center of clusterAnatomical region
Visual–occipitalHyper > Aphan−28, +56, +2Left BA10
+2, −92, −24Cerebellum
+40, +32, +38Right BA9
−24, +32, +36Left BA9
+8, +46, −12Right BA11
Hippocampus-leftHyper > Aphan−2, −24, −54Brain stem
Frontal parietal-leftAphan > Hyper−2, +56, −24Left BA11
Left dorsal attention IPS (intraparietal sulcus)Aphan > Hyper−32, +22, +38Middle frontal gyrus
Hippocampus-leftAphan > Hyper−10, −2, +40Left BA24
Left dorsal attention IPSAphan > Control+6, +36, +56Right BA8
Hippocampus-leftAphan > Control−42, +40, +22Left BA10
NetworkComparisonMNI coordinates of center of clusterAnatomical region
Visual–occipitalHyper > Aphan−28, +56, +2Left BA10
+2, −92, −24Cerebellum
+40, +32, +38Right BA9
−24, +32, +36Left BA9
+8, +46, −12Right BA11
Hippocampus-leftHyper > Aphan−2, −24, −54Brain stem
Frontal parietal-leftAphan > Hyper−2, +56, −24Left BA11
Left dorsal attention IPS (intraparietal sulcus)Aphan > Hyper−32, +22, +38Middle frontal gyrus
Hippocampus-leftAphan > Hyper−10, −2, +40Left BA24
Left dorsal attention IPSAphan > Control+6, +36, +56Right BA8
Hippocampus-leftAphan > Control−42, +40, +22Left BA10

Note: The table identifies networks more strongly connected in the hyperphantasic or aphantasic group and the targets of differential connectivity

Figure 4

Areas more strongly activated in the hyperphantasic and control groups than the aphantasic group (left and right columns, respectively) in a subtraction of the perception from the visualization conditions.

Table 7

Task-based fMRI: areas more strongly activated in hyperphantasic and control than aphantasic participants in a comparison of visualization with perception

Group comparisonTask comparisonCluster center coordinates (MNI space)Cluster center regionCluster size (number of voxels)Z Scores
Hyperphantasia > AphantasiaImagination-perception−45 −22 62Left anterior parietal4115.25
−9 −7 5Thalamus564.45
18 –61 −55Cerebellum (anterior)564.43
33 –28 35Right anterior parietal454.22
21 −52 −25Cerebellum (posterior)344.83
Control > AphantasiaImagination-perception−39 −31 50Left anterior parietal2255.52
Group comparisonTask comparisonCluster center coordinates (MNI space)Cluster center regionCluster size (number of voxels)Z Scores
Hyperphantasia > AphantasiaImagination-perception−45 −22 62Left anterior parietal4115.25
−9 −7 5Thalamus564.45
18 –61 −55Cerebellum (anterior)564.43
33 –28 35Right anterior parietal454.22
21 −52 −25Cerebellum (posterior)344.83
Control > AphantasiaImagination-perception−39 −31 50Left anterior parietal2255.52

Table 7

Task-based fMRI: areas more strongly activated in hyperphantasic and control than aphantasic participants in a comparison of visualization with perception

Group comparisonTask comparisonCluster center coordinates (MNI space)Cluster center regionCluster size (number of voxels)Z Scores
Hyperphantasia > AphantasiaImagination-perception−45 −22 62Left anterior parietal4115.25
−9 −7 5Thalamus564.45
18 –61 −55Cerebellum (anterior)564.43
33 –28 35Right anterior parietal454.22
21 −52 −25Cerebellum (posterior)344.83
Control > AphantasiaImagination-perception−39 −31 50Left anterior parietal2255.52
Group comparisonTask comparisonCluster center coordinates (MNI space)Cluster center regionCluster size (number of voxels)Z Scores
Hyperphantasia > AphantasiaImagination-perception−45 −22 62Left anterior parietal4115.25
−9 −7 5Thalamus564.45
18 –61 −55Cerebellum (anterior)564.43
33 –28 35Right anterior parietal454.22
21 −52 −25Cerebellum (posterior)344.83
Control > AphantasiaImagination-perception−39 −31 50Left anterior parietal2255.52

Volumetric MRI

No significant differences in brain region volumes were found between the three groups.

Diffusion Tensor Imaging

No significant differences in fractional anisotropy were detected between groups for any brain regions.

Discussion

Our study is the first wide-ranging exploration of the neuropsychological and neural associations of aphantasia and hyperphantasia. In keeping with their subjective reports (Dawes etal. 2020; Zeman etal. 2020), participants from these groups are distinguished by their scores on measures of autobiographical memory, future and atemporal imagination, and face recognition, but not by standard measures of anterograde memory. rsfMRI reveals stronger connectivity between the posterior visual network and prefrontal regions among people with hyperphantasia than those with aphantasia. In a comparison of visualization to perception, we found higher levels of brain activation centered on the anterior parietal lobe among people with hyperphantasia and control participants than in those with aphantasia. Aphantasia is associated with autistic spectrum traits and introversion, hyperphantasia with “openness.” Classic “visual imagery tasks” discriminate poorly between people with aphantasia and hyperphantasia. We discuss each of these findings in turn.

We predicted that aphantasia would be associated with relatively impoverished autobiographical memory, while hyperphantasia would be associated with enriched autobiographical memory, on the basis of our previous questionnaire data (Zeman etal. 2020). Despite a modest but statistically significant group difference in IQ in favor of the aphantasic group and essentially equivalent performance in the two groups on standard measures of anterograde recall and recognition memory for visual and verbal material, this prediction was strongly confirmed. Given the evidence that autobiographical memory performance is related to performance on measures of atemporal and future-directed imagination, and anecdotal evidence from some of our participants of difficulty with future thinking, we predicted that there would be similar differences in this domain. This prediction was also confirmed.

Autobiographical recollection, like the imagination tasks used in this study, requires participants to project themselves into mentally constructed scenes, engaging abilities that have been variously described in terms of “autonoesis” (Tulving 1985), scene-construction (Hassabis and Maguire 2007), self-projection (Buckner and Carroll 2007), and constructive episodic simulation (Schacter and Addis 2020). Such tasks make different demands to laboratory measures of recall and recognition for recently encountered material, helping to explain the dissociation evident here between performance on these two forms of memory test (Gilboa 2004; Palombo etal. 2018). The markedly contrasting performance seen in these tasks among participants at the extremes of the visual imagery vividness spectrum is in keeping with the more modest correlations between imagery vividness and the richness of autobiographical memory revealed in samples with typical imagery vividness (Vannucci etal. 2016; Palombo etal. 2018).

Why should extreme variations in imagery vividness be associated with such marked variations in autobiographical recollection and imagination? Both recollection and imagination typically involve evocation of sensory details—a “reexperiencing,” constrained by a past episode in the case of autobiographical memory, more autonomous in the cause of imagination (Palombo etal. 2018; Schacter and Addis 2020). The absence of the ability to evoke sensory details in this way, whether in the visual modality alone or across all sensory modalities (Dawes etal. 2020; Zeman etal. 2020), might be expected to have a profound impact on these cognitive processes. Clinical reports linking cortical visual impairment to autobiographical memory deficits (Rubin and Greenberg 1998; Gardini etal. 2011; Ahmed etal. 2018), taken together with evidence for visual memory impairment in the recently described Syndrome of Severely Deficient Autobiographical Memory (Palombo etal. 2015), are broadly consistent with our results. We consider the neural basis for this key finding below.

We predicted that we would find evidence of face recognition difficulty among participants with aphantasia, but not hyperphantasia, on the basis of subjective reports (Zeman etal. 2020). The significant elevation of PI scores among aphantasic participants supports this prediction. Although it is in keeping with an independent observation of an association between prosopagnosia and low imagery vividness scores (Gruter etal. 2009), we regard this finding as tentative. The PI is a self-report measure, albeit a validated one, and it could be that weak facial imagery has a metacognitive effect, reducing the confidence of people with aphantasia in their ability to recognize faces. Our measure of famous face recognition showed a ceiling effect and was therefore not an adequate objective measure. The contrast between the performance of the aphantasic and hyperphantasic groups on the Famous Building Test could reflect a perceptual or semantic effect and, like face recognition ability itself, requires further exploration.

Given that people with lifelong aphantasia do not describe major perceptual difficulties yet are unable to summon images to the mind’s eye voluntarily, reduction in connectivity between relevant cognitive control systems and visual cortices offers a plausible neural mechanism. The underlying thought here is that reverse activation of visual cortices in the service of visual imagery is likely to place stronger demands on reverse connections between frontoparietal executive regions and visual cortices than does visual perception (Dijkstra etal. 2019). This idea is strengthened by the observation that many people with aphantasia experience visual dreams (Dawes etal. 2020; Zeman etal. 2020), suggesting that they are capable of experiencing imagery when the requirement for voluntary imagery generation is removed. Our finding of reduced resting state connectivity between several areas of prefrontal cortex and the visual–occipital network among people with aphantasia, compared with those with hyperphantasia, and to control participants, supports an explanation along these lines. Comparable differences in connectivity have been reported in relation to autobiographical memory: Stronger resting state connectivity between the medial temporal lobes and visual cortices was seen in individuals reporting a more “episodic” style of autobiographical recollection, involving greater event detail, while individuals with a more factual style of recollection showed stronger connectivity between the medial temporal lobes and inferior and middle prefrontal regions (Sheldon etal. 2016). In a related finding, among patients with Parkinson’s disease, there is a stronger association between mind-wandering frequency and coupling between the default mode network and visual areas among patients with hallucinations as a manifestation of their disorder than among those who do not hallucinate (Walpola etal. 2020). Our observation of stronger resting state coupling between both lateral and medial prefrontal regions and visual cortices among participants with hyperphantasia than among participants with aphantasia is potentially relevant both to the difference in their subjective experience of imagery and to the differences in performance we report on measures of autobiographical memory and imagination.

Using our functional imaging paradigm, we did not find any difference between aphantasic and hyperphantasic groups in the most straightforward comparison of visualization versus control conditions in these two groups, in conflict with our initial hypothesis. However, we observed an activation difference in the anterior parietal cortex between hyperphantasic and control participants on the one hand and aphantasic participants on the other, in a comparison of visualization and perception. The anterior parietal area concerned, which was bilateral in the hyperphantasic/aphantasic comparison but larger in the left than right hemisphere, is close, but slightly anterior, to the area of maximum activation by visual imagery tasks in a recent meta-analysis (Winlove etal. 2018). We hypothesize that this activation difference reflects weaker deployment of visual attention within the parietal lobe during attempted visual imagery in our aphantasic participants than in hyperphantasic and control participants.

A number of aphantasic participants in our questionnaire study (Zeman etal. 2020) had mentioned spontaneously that they were on the autistic spectrum. Given that deficits in imagination, broadly construed, have been among the defining features of autistic spectrum disorder, this association appeared plausible. It is consistent with the observation from our questionnaire study of an association between aphantasia and mathematical and scientific occupations (Zeman etal. 2020), as these are associated, at a group level, with autistic traits (Baron-Cohen etal. 2001). The current study provides tentative support for a link between aphantasia and autistic spectrum features, with elevated Autism Spectrum Quotient questionnaire scores among aphantasic participants in comparison to the control group. This observation may relate to the rsfMRI findings discussed above, as autism has been associated with reduced long-range brain connectivity (Lai etal. 2014).

The association revealed by the NEO-FFI of aphantasia with reduced extraversion is in keeping with its association with elevated AQ scores, as introversion is itself associated with elevated AQ scores (Wakabayashi etal. 2006). In contrast, we found that hyperphantasia was associated with increased levels of openness. This construct is more fully described as reflecting “an openness to new experiences, broad interests, and an active imagination, and a likelihood of experiencing both positive and negative emotions more keenly than most people.” It is an intuitively plausible personality correlate for high levels of imagery vividness and has been linked to efficient information processing with the default mode network (Beaty etal. 2016) and, tentatively, to variation in the dopamine D4 receptor gene (DRD4) and the catechol-O-methyltransferase gene (COMT) (Deyoung etal. 2011).

We included three “standard” imagery tests as they have often been used as measures of imagery, despite doubts that they would be revealing in this context. The first two tests, the Manikins test and curved segments test, showed ceiling effects in our study, with scores over 90% correct in all three groups on the former and over 98% correct on the latter. They have other shortcomings in the context of visual imagery vividness extremes, as they primarily assess “spatial imagery,” which may be preserved in aphantasia (Keogh and Pearson 2018; Bainbridge etal. 2020; Dawes etal. 2020), rather than “object imagery,” which is likely to be the main locus of impairment. The animal tails test is, in contrast, directed to object imagery (Farah etal. 1988), but our previously described patient MX, with acquired aphantasia, scored normally on this test on the basis of “knowledge” rather than visualization (Zeman etal. 2010). The reduction of scores in the aphantasic group in the current study suggests that conscious visual imagery supports performance to some degree, although their 82% correct average score indicates, in keeping with the result in MX’s case, that other sources of knowledge make a contribution. More generally, these findings show that results of “imagery tests” must be interpreted cautiously, in the light of participants’ reports of their experience, as they are by no means pure “measures of visual imagery”: Other sources of knowledge, both general semantic knowledge and extravisual but modality-specific information, for example, kinesthetic imagery, can influence performance. Moreover, visual imagery, itself, is heterogeneous, incorporating “object” and “spatial” imagery, which appear likely to have distinct behavioral associations and neural bases (Blazhenkova and Kozhevnikov 2010; Kozhevnikov and Blazhenkova 2013).

Our study has a number of limitations. First, given the complexity of the neural network subserving visualization, and variability in the associated features of aphantasia (Zeman etal. 2020), it is likely that aphantasia is heterogeneous, with several subtypes: For example, we suspect that aphantasia in association with prosopagnosia will turn out to be distinct from aphantasia occurring in its absence. Hyperphantasia, also, is likely to be heterogeneous. In this study, we have not attempted to tease apart possible subtypes. We were concerned that resulting “noise” might obscure any neuropsychological and neural associations of extreme imagery. Our findings suggest, however, that there are at least some consistent differences between people at the two imagery vividness extremes. Second, anecdotal, and some experimental, evidence (Morina etal. 2013) indicate that imagery extremes are likely to have affective associations. We have not explored these in this study: This topic will be a fruitful area for further research (Holmes and Mathews 2010; Palombo and Levine 2018; Dawes etal. 2020). Third, while we found the classical “imagery tests” relatively insensitive to visual imagery vividness extremes, as discussed above, the measurement of response times, which we did not attempt, might have been more informative. Fourth, the IQ discrepancy between our aphantasic and hyperphantasic groups introduces a complexity into the interpretation of our results. However, as the discrepancy favored the aphantasic group, in which we found that autobiographical memory and imagination scores were reduced, if anything, differences between the groups in these respects are likely to have been underestimated. Fifth, we did not monitor eye movements during MRI scanning. This has not been undertaken, as a rule, in functional imaging studies of visual imagery, but future studies should ideally include this control, in case systematic group differences in eye movements may be influencing brain imaging findings. Sixth, in future studies, it would be desirable to include a control condition, such as picture description, to probe the possibility that the reduced scores on measure of autobiographical memory and imagination in the aphantasic group are related to a pervasive difference in narrative production. Finally, our use of a relatively low-powered scanner (1.5T) and whole brain analyses makes it likely that we have detected only a minority of the structural and functional differences between the brains of people lying at the two visual imagery vividness extremes. Specifically, the current study has not applied a region of interest analysis to address the question of whether there are differences in the area of early visual cortices between participants at the two extremes of the vividness spectrum, as predicted by the findings of Bergmann etal. (2016). We plan to address this question in future work.

In conclusion, aphantasia and hyperphantasia are recently described extremes of visual imagery vividness, occurring in around 1% and 3% of the population, respectively. Spontaneous self-report and questionnaire data from several thousand participants have suggested that these imagery vividness extremes have a range of psychological and occupational associations (Dawes etal. 2020; Zeman etal. 2020). Here, we have shown that aphantasia is not associated with a reduction in performance on standard declarative memory tests. However, in keeping with self-report data (Zeman etal. 2020), it is associated with a marked reduction in performance on exacting measures of autobiographical memory and temporal and atemporal “imagination.” Also, in keeping with the self-report data, there are tentative associations between aphantasia and both face recognition difficulty and autistic spectrum traits. Hyperphantasia is associated with the personality trait of openness, while aphantasia is associated with a reduction in extraversion. rsfMRI reveals stronger functional connectivity between prefrontal regions and the visual–occipital network among hyperphantasic than aphantasic participants. Task-based fMRI identifies an anterior parietal region distinguishing the aphantasic sample from the control and hyperphantasic groups. Further work is required to explore the cognitive and behavioral advantages and disadvantages of what we suspect to be two key, contrasting, modes of human information processing, highlighted by the comparison of hyperphantasia with aphantasia: One more episodic and sensorily-rich, the other more semantic and factual. We anticipate that imagery extremes will have affective associations, which we have not studied here. A broader range of approaches to behavioral assessment and the use of more powerful imaging modalities have the potential to shed further light on this fundamental contrast.

Notes

We are very grateful to the participants in this study. We thank Holly Wilson, Rachel Johnson, Amy Lewins, Kate Mitchell, and Sarah Mathew for help in collecting the brain imaging data, and Mada Radu for help with collecting behavioral data and data curation. We would also like to thank The NIHR Exeter Clinical Research Facility and the EXTEND project (https://exetercrfnihr.org/about/exeter-10000/) for assisting us with the recruitment of participants for this study. Conflict of Interest:

The authors declare no competing interests.

Funding

United Kingdom Arts and Humanities Research Council Science in Culture Innovation Award: The Eye’s Mind—a study of the neural basis of visual imagination and its role in culture (AH/M002756/1); Follow-on Funding Award: Extreme Imagination in Mind, Brain and Culture (AH/R004684/1).

Author Contributions

Conceptualization: Zeman; Data curation: Dance, Gaddum, Heuerman-Williamson, Jones, Zeman; Formal analysis: Fulford, Milton; Funding acquisition: Mackisack, Winlove, Zeman; Investigation: Dance, Fulford, Gaddum, Jones, Knight, Milton, Heuerman-Williamson, Jones; Methodology: Dance, Fulford, Gaddum, Milton, Heuerman-Williamson, Jones, Winlove, Zeman; Project administration: Winlove, Zeman; Resources: Zeman; Supervision: Fulford, Milton, Winlove, Zeman; Validation: Fulford, Milton, Zeman; Visualization: Fulford, Milton; Writing—original draft: Zeman; Writing—review and editing: all authors.

References

Ahmed

S

,

Irish

M

,

Loane

C

,

Baker

I

,

Husain

M

,

Thompson

S

,

Blanco-Duque

C

,

Mackay

C

,

Zamboni

G

,

Foxe

D

, et al.

2018

.

Association between precuneus volume and autobiographical memory impairment in posterior cortical atrophy: beyond the visual syndrome

.

Neuroimage Clin

.

18

:

822

834

.

Andersson

JLR

,

Jenkinson

M

,

Smith

S.

2007a

.

Non-linear Optimisation

.

FMRIB technical report TR07JA1

. www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/analysis/techrep.

Andersson

JLR

,

Jenkinson

M

,

Smith

S.

2007b

.

Non-linear Registration, AKA Spatial Normalisation

.

FMRIB technical report TR07JA2

. www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/analysis/techrep.

Ashburner

J

.

2007

.

A fast diffeomorphic image registration algorithm

.

NeuroImage

.

38

(

1

):

95

113

.

Bainbridge W, Pounder Z, Eardley A, Baker C.

2021

.

Quantifying aphantasia through drawing: Those without visual imagery show deficits in object but not spatial memory

.

Cortex

.

135

:

159

177

.

Barnett

KJ

,

Newell

FN

.

2008

.

Synaesthesia is associated with enhanced, self-rated visual imagery

.

Conscious Cogn

.

17

(

3

):

1032

1039

.

Baron-Cohen

S

,

Wheelwright

S

,

Skinner

R

,

Martin

J

,

Clubley

E

.

2001

.

The autism-spectrum quotient (AQ): evidence from Asperger syndrome/high-functioning autism, males and females, scientists and mathematicians

.

J Autism Dev Disord

.

31

(

1

):

5

17

.

Bartolomeo

P

.

2008

.

The neural correlates of visual mental imagery: an ongoing debate

.

Cortex

44

(

2

):

107

108

.

Beaty

RE

,

Kaufman

SB

,

Benedek

M

,

Jung

RE

,

Kenett

YN

,

Jauk

E

,

Neubauer

AC

,

Silvia

PJ

.

2016

.

Personality and complex brain networks: the role of openness to experience in default network efficiency

.

Hum Brain Mapp

.

37

(

2

):

773

779

.

Behrmann

M

,

Moscovitch

M

,

Winocur

G

.

1994

.

Intact visual imagery and impaired visual perception in a patient with visual agnosia

.

J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform

.

20

(

5

):

1068

1087

.

Bergmann

J

,

Genc

E

,

Kohler

A

,

Singer

W

,

Pearson

J

.

2016

.

Smaller primary visual cortex is associated with stronger, but less precise mental imagery

.

Cereb Cortex

.

26

(

9

):

3838

3850

.

Blazhenkova

O

,

Kozhevnikov

M

.

2010

.

Visual-object ability: a new dimension of non-verbal intelligence

.

Cognition

.

117

(

3

):

276

301

.

Breedlove

JL

,

St-Yves

G

,

Olman

CA

,

Naselaris

T

.

2020

.

Generative feedback explains distinct brain activity codes for seen and mental images

.

Curr Biol

.

30

(

12

):

2211

2224.e6

. doi:

.

Buckner

RL

,

Carroll

DC

.

2007

.

Self-projection and the brain

.

Trends Cogn Sci

.

11

(

2

):

49

57

.

Dance CJ, Jaquiery M, Eagleman DM, Porteous D, Zeman A, Simner J.

2021

.

What is the relationship between Aphantasia, Synaesthesia and Autism?

Conscious Cogn.

89

:103087.

Google Scholar

OpenURL Placeholder Text

D'Argembeau

A

,

Van der Linden

M

.

2006

.

Individual differences in the phenomenology of mental time travel: the effect of vivid visual imagery and emotion regulation strategies

.

Conscious Cogn

.

15

(

2

):

342

350

.

Dawes

AJ

,

Keogh

R

,

Andrillon

T

,

Pearson

J

.

2020

.

A cognitive profile of multi-sensory imagery, memory and dreaming in aphantasia

.

Sci Rep

.

10

(

1

):

10022

.

Desikan

RS

,

Segonne

F

,

Fischl

B

,

Quinn

BT

,

Dickerson

BC

,

Blacker

D

,

Buckner

RL

,

Dale

AM

,

Maguire

RP

,

Hyman

BT

, et al.

2006

.

An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest

.

NeuroImage

.

31

(

3

):

968

980

.

Deyoung

CG

,

Cicchetti

D

,

Rogosch

FA

,

Gray

JR

,

Eastman

M

,

Grigorenko

EL

.

2011

.

Sources of cognitive exploration: genetic variation in the prefrontal dopamine system predicts openness/intellect

.

J Res Pers

.

45

(

4

):

364

371

.

Dijkstra

N

,

Bosch

SE

,

van

Gerven

MAJ

.

2019

.

Shared neural mechanisms of visual perception and Imagery

.

Trends Cogn Sci

.

23

(

5

):

423

434

.

Evans

JJ

,

Heggs

AJ

,

Antoun

N

,

Hodges

JR

.

1995

.

Progressive prosopagnosia associated with selective right temporal lobe atrophy. A new syndrome?

Brain

.

118

(

Pt 1

):

1

13

.

Google Scholar

OpenURL Placeholder Text

Farah

MJ

.

1984

.

The neurological basis of mental imagery: a componential analysis

.

Cognition

.

18

(

1–3

):

245

272

.

Google Scholar

OpenURL Placeholder Text

Farah

MJ

,

Hammond

KM

,

Levine

DN

,

Calvanio

R

.

1988

.

Visual and mental imagery: dissociable systems of representation

.

Cogn Psychol

.

20

(

4

):

439

462

.

Faw

B

.

2009

.

Conflicting intuitions may be based on differing abilities—evidence from mental imaging research

.

J Conscious Stud

.

16

:

45

68

.

Google Scholar

OpenURL Placeholder Text

Friston

K

,

Adams

RA

,

Perrinet

L

,

Breakspear

M

.

2012

.

Perceptions as hypotheses: saccades as experiments

.

Front Psychol

.

3

:151. doi:

.

Google Scholar

OpenURL Placeholder Text

Crossref

Fulford

J

,

Milton

F

,

Salas

D

,

Smith

A

,

Simler

A

,

Winlove

C

,

Zeman

A

.

2018

.

The neural correlates of visual imagery vividness—an fMRI study and literature review

.

Cortex

.

105

:

26

40

.

Galton

F

.

1880

.

Statistics of mental imagery

.

Mind

.

5

:

301

318

.

Google Scholar

OpenURL Placeholder Text

Gardini

S

,

Concari

L

,

Pagliara

S

,

Ghetti

C

,

Venneri

A

,

Caffarra

P

.

2011

.

Visuo-spatial imagery impairment in posterior cortical atrophy: a cognitive and SPECT study

.

Behav Neurol

.

24

(

2

):

123

132

.

Gilboa

A

.

2004

.

Autobiographical and episodic memory—one and the same? Evidence from prefrontal activation in neuroimaging studies

.

Neuropsychologia

.

42

(

10

):

1336

1349

.

Gilboa

A

,

Winocur

G

,

Grady

CL

,

Hevenor

SJ

,

Moscovitch

M

.

2004

.

Remembering our past: functional neuroanatomy of recollection of recent and very remote personal events

.

Cereb Cortex

.

14

(

11

):

1214

1225

.

Greenberg

DL

,

Knowlton

BJ

.

2014

.

The role of visual imagery in autobiographical memory

.

Mem Cogn

.

42

(

6

):

922

934

.

Gruter

T

,

Gruter

M

,

Bell

V

,

Carbon

CC

.

2009

.

Visual mental imagery in congenital prosopagnosia

.

Neurosci Lett

.

453

(

3

):

135

140

.

Hassabis

D

,

Kumaran

D

,

Vann

SD

,

Maguire

EA

.

2007

.

Patients with hippocampal amnesia cannot imagine new experiences

.

Proc Natl Acad Sci USA

.

104

(

5

):

1726

1731

.

Hassabis

D

,

Maguire

EA

.

2007

.

Deconstructing episodic memory with construction

.

Trends Cogn Sci

11

(

7

):

299

306

.

Holmes

EA

,

Mathews

A

.

2010

.

Mental imagery in emotion and emotional disorders

.

Clin Psychol Rev

.

30

(

3

):

349

362

.

Jacobs

C

,

Schwarzkopf

DS

,

Silvanto

J

.

2018

.

Visual working memory performance in aphantasia

.

Cortex

.

105

:

61

73

.

Keogh

R

,

Bergmann

J

,

Pearson

J

.

2020

.

Cortical excitability controls the strength of mental imagery

.

elife

.

9

:

e50232

.

Keogh

R

,

Pearson

J

.

2018

.

The blind mind: no sensory visual imagery in aphantasia

.

Cortex.

105

:

53

60

.

Kiebel

SJ

,

Poline

JB

,

Friston

KJ

,

Holmes

AP

,

Worsley

KJ

.

1999

.

Robust smoothness estimation in statistical parametric maps using standardized residuals from the general linear model

.

NeuroImage

.

10

(

6

):

756

766

.

Kosslyn

S

,

Thompson

W

,

Ganis

G

.

2006

.

The case for mental imagery

.

Oxford

:

Oxford University Press

.

Kozhevnikov

M

,

Blazhenkova

O

.

2013

. Individual differences in object versus spatial imagery: from neural correlates to real-word applications. In:

Lacey

S

,

Lawson

R

(eds).

Multisensory imagery

.

New York

:

Springer

. pp.

299

318

.

Lai

MC

,

Lombardo

MV

,

Baron-Cohen

S

.

2014

.

Autism

.

Lancet

.

383

(

9920

):

896

910

.

Levine

B

,

Svoboda

E

,

Hay

JF

,

Winocur

G

,

Moscovitch

M

.

2002

.

Aging and autobiographical memory: dissociating episodic from semantic retrieval

.

Psychol Aging

.

17

(

4

):

677

689

.

MacKisack M, Aldworth S, Macpherson F, Onians J, Winlove C, Zeman A.

2016

.

On picturing a candle: The prehistory of imagery science

.

Front Psychol

.

7

:

515

.

Google Scholar

OpenURL Placeholder Text

Marks

DF

.

1973

.

Visual imagery differences in the recall of pictures

.

Br J Psychol

.

64

(

1

):

17

24

.

McCrae

RR

,

Costa

PT

.

2010

. NEO inventories for the NEO personality inventory-3 (NEO-PI-3), NEO five-factor inventory-3 (NEO-FFI-3). In:

NEO personality inventory-revised (NEO PI-R): professional manual

. Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc., Lutz.

Milton

F

,

Muhlert

N

,

Pindus

DM

,

Butler

CR

,

Kapur

N

,

Graham

KS

,

Zeman

A

.

2010

.

Remote memory deficits in transient epileptic amnesia

.

Brain

.

133

(

5

):

1368

1379

.

Morina

N

,

Leibold

E

,

Ehring

T

.

2013

.

Vividness of general mental imagery is associated with the occurrence of intrusive memories

.

J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry

.

44

(

2

):

221

226

.

Munzert

J

,

Lorey

B

.

2013

. Motor and visual imagery in sports. In:

Lacey

S

,

Lawson

R

(eds).

Multisensory imagery

.

New York

:

Springer

. pp.

319

341

.

Osterreith

P

,

Rey

A

.

1944

.

Le test de copie d'une figure complexe

.

Arch Psychol

.

30

:

205

220

.

Google Scholar

OpenURL Placeholder Text

Palombo

DJ

,

Alain

C

,

Soderlund

H

,

Khuu

W

,

Levine

B

.

2015

.

Severely deficient autobiographical memory (SDAM) in healthy adults: a new mnemonic syndrome

.

Neuropsychologia

72

:

105

118

.

Palombo

DJ

,

Sheldon

S

,

Levine

B

.

2018

.

Individual differences in autobiographical memory

.

Trends Cogn Sci

.

22

(

7

):

583

597

.

Pearson

J

.

2019

.

The human imagination: the cognitive neuroscience of visual mental imagery

.

Nat Rev Neurosci

.

20

(

10

):

624

634

.

Ratcliff

G

.

1979

.

Spatial thought, mental rotation and the right cerebral hemisphere

.

Neuropsychologia

.

17

(

1

):

49

54

.

Rubin

DC

,

Greenberg

DL

.

1998

.

Visual memory-deficit amnesia: a distinct amnesic presentation and etiology

.

Proc Natl Acad Sci USA

.

95

(

9

):

5413

5416

.

Rueckert

D

,

Sonoda

LI

,

Hayes

C

,

Hill

DLG

,

Leach

MO

,

Hawkes

DJ

.

1999

.

Non-rigid registration using free-form deformations: application to breast MR images

.

IEEE Trans Med Imaging

.

18

(

8

):

712

721

.

Schacter

DL

,

Addis

DR

.

2007

.

The cognitive neuroscience of constructive memory: remembering the past and imagining the future

.

Philos Trans R Soc Lond Ser B Biol Sci

.

362

(

1481

):

773

786

.

Schacter

DL

,

Addis

DR

.

2020

. Memory and imagination: perspectives on constructive episodic simulation. In:

Abraham

A

(ed).

The Cambridge handbook of the imagination

.

Cambridge

:

Cambridge University Press

. pp.

111

131

.

Shah

P

,

Gaule

A

,

Sowden

S

,

Bird

G

,

Cook

R

.

2015

.

The 20-item prosopagnosia index (PI20): a self-report instrument for identifying developmental prosopagnosia

.

R Soc Open Sci

.

2

(

6

):

140343

.

Sheldon

S

,

Farb

N

,

Palombo

DJ

,

Levine

B

.

2016

.

Intrinsic medial temporal lobe connectivity relates to individual differences in episodic autobiographical remembering

.

Cortex

74

:

206

216

.

Shepard

R

.

1988

. The imagination of the scientist. In:

Egan

K

,

Nadaner

D

(eds).

Imagination and education

.

Milton Keynes

:

Open University Press

. pp.

153

185

.

Google Scholar

OpenURL Placeholder Text

Smallwood

J

,

Schooler

JW

.

2015

.

The science of mind wandering: empirically navigating the stream of consciousness

.

Annu Rev Psychol

.

66

(

1

):

487

518

.

Smith

SM

,

Jenkinson

M

,

Johansen-Berg

H

,

Rueckert

D

,

Nichols

TE

,

Mackay

CE

,

Watkins

KE

,

Ciccarelli

O

,

Cader

MZ

,

Matthews

PM

, et al.

2006

.

Tract-based spatial statistics: voxelwise analysis of multi-subject diffusion data

.

NeuroImage

.

31

(

4

):

1487

1505

.

Smith

SM

,

Jenkinson

M

,

Woolrich

MW

,

Beckmann

CF

,

Behrens

TEJ

,

Johansen-Berg

H

,

Bannister

PR

,

De Luca

M

,

Drobnjak

I

,

Flitney

DE

, et al.

2004

.

Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL

.

NeuroImage

23

(

S1

):

208

219

.

Google Scholar

OpenURL Placeholder Text

Smith

SM

.

2002

.

Fast robust automated brain extraction

.

Hum Brain Mapp

.

17

(

3

):

143

155

.

Song

XW

,

Dong

ZY

,

Long

XY

,

Li

SF

,

Zuo

XN

,

Zhu

CZ

, et al.

2011

.

REST: a toolkit for resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging data processing

.

PLoS One

.

6

:e25031.

Google Scholar

OpenURL Placeholder Text

Stark

CE

,

Squire

LR

.

2001

.

When zero is not zero: the problem of ambiguous baseline conditions in fMRI

.

Proc Natl Acad Sci USA

.

98

(

22

):

12760

12766

.

Steinberg

BA

,

Bieliauskas

LA

,

Smith

GE

,

Ivnik

RJ

.

2005

.

Mayo's older Americans normative studies: age- and IQ-adjusted norms for the trail-making test, the stroop test, and MAE controlled oral word association test

.

Clin Neuropsychol

.

19

(

3–4

):

329

377

.

Google Scholar

OpenURL Placeholder Text

Tulving

E

.

1985

.

Memory and consciousness

.

Can Psychol

.

26

(

1

):

1

12

.

Tzourio-Mazoyer

N

,

Landeau

B

,

Papathanassiou

D

,

Crivello

F

,

Etard

O

,

Delcroix

N

,

Mazoyer

B

,

Joliot

M

.

2002

.

Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain

.

NeuroImage

15

(

1

):

273

289

.

Van der Meulen

M

,

Logie

RH

,

Della Sala

S

.

2009

.

Selective interference with image retention and generation: evidence for the workspace model

.

Q J Exp Psychol (Hove)

.

62

(

8

):

1568

1580

.

Vannucci

M

,

Pelagatti

C

,

Chiorri

C

,

Mazzoni

G

.

2016

.

Visual object imagery and autobiographical memory: object imagers are better at remembering their personal past

.

Memory

.

24

(

4

):

455

470

.

Wakabayashi A, Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S.

2006

.

Are autistic traits an independent personality dimension? A study of the Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ) and the NEO-PI-R

.

Pers Individ Dif

.

45

(

5

):

873

883

.

Google Scholar

OpenURL Placeholder Text

Walpola

IC

,

Muller

AJ

,

Hall

JM

,

Andrews-Hanna

JR

,

Irish

M

,

Lewis

SJG

,

Shine

JM

,

O'Callaghan

C

.

2020

.

Mind-wandering in Parkinson's disease hallucinations reflects primary visual and default network coupling

.

Cortex

125

:

233

245

.

Warrington

EK

.

1984

.

Recognition memory test (RMT)

.

Windsor

:

NFER-Nelson

.

Google Scholar

OpenURL Placeholder Text

Wechsler

D

.

1997

.

Wechsler memory scale III

.

San Antonio (TX)

:

The Psychological Corporation

.

Google Scholar

OpenURL Placeholder Text

Wechsler

D.

2008

.

WAIS-IV: Wechsler adult intelligence scale

.

Psychological Corporation & Pearson Education Inc.

, San Antonio, TX.

Google Scholar

OpenURL Placeholder Text

Wechsler

D

.

2011

.

Wechsler abbreviated scale of intelligence

.

San Antonio (TX)

:

Psychological Corporation

.

Google Scholar

OpenURL Placeholder Text

Whitfield-Gabrieli

S

,

Nieto-Castanon

A

.

2012

.

Conn: a functional connectivity toolbox for correlated and anticorrelated brain networks

.

Brain Connect

.

2

(

3

):

125

141

.

Wicken

M

,

Keogh

R

,

Pearson

J

.

2021

.

The critical role of mental imagery in human emotion: insights from fear-based imagery and aphantasia

.

Proc R Soc B.

288

:20210267.

Google Scholar

OpenURL Placeholder Text

Winlove

CIP

,

Milton

F

,

Ranson

J

,

Fulford

J

,

MacKisack

M

,

Macpherson

F

,

Zeman

A

.

2018

.

The neural correlates of visual imagery: a co-ordinate-based meta-analysis

.

Cortex

.

105

:

4

25

.

Zeman

A

,

Dewar

M

,

Della Sala

S

.

2015

.

Lives without imagery—congenital aphantasia

.

Cortex

73

:

378

380

.

Zeman

A

,

Milton

F

,

Della Sala

S

,

Dewar

M

,

Frayling

T

,

Gaddum

J

,

Hattersley

A

,

Heuerman-Williamson

B

,

Jones

K

,

MacKisack

M

, et al.

2020

.

Phantasia—the psychological significance of lifelong visual imagery vividness extremes

.

Cortex

130

:

426

440

.

Zeman

A

,

Della Sala

S

,

Torrens

LA

,

Gountouna

VE

,

McGonigle

DJ

,

Logie

RH

.

2010

.

Loss of imagery phenomenology with intact visuo-spatial task performance: a case of 'blind imagination

.

Neuropsychologia

48

(

1

):

145

155

.

Zigmond

AS

,

Snaith

RP

.

1983

.

The hospital anxiety and depression scale

.

Acta Psychiatr Scand

.

67

(

6

):

361

370

.

Author notes

Fraser Milton, Jon Fulford and Adam Zeman contributed equally to this work

© The Author(s) 2021. Published by Oxford University Press.

This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted reuse, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Issue Section:

Original Article

Advertisem*nt

Citations

Views

47,492

Altmetric

More metrics information

Metrics

Total Views 47,492

41,299 Pageviews

6,193 PDF Downloads

Since 5/1/2021

Month: Total Views:
May 2021 59
June 2021 3,439
July 2021 1,346
August 2021 1,132
September 2021 1,788
October 2021 1,434
November 2021 1,840
December 2021 1,450
January 2022 1,592
February 2022 1,441
March 2022 2,424
April 2022 1,619
May 2022 1,053
June 2022 908
July 2022 1,310
August 2022 1,000
September 2022 1,029
October 2022 1,290
November 2022 1,674
December 2022 1,348
January 2023 2,019
February 2023 1,187
March 2023 1,348
April 2023 1,194
May 2023 1,068
June 2023 729
July 2023 915
August 2023 984
September 2023 1,050
October 2023 1,393
November 2023 1,032
December 2023 921
January 2024 1,296
February 2024 1,823
March 2024 1,357

Citations

Powered by Dimensions

Altmetrics

×

Citing articles via

Advertisem*nt

Behavioral and Neural Signatures of Visual Imagery Vividness Extremes: Aphantasia versus Hyperphantasia (2024)
Top Articles
Latest Posts
Article information

Author: Stevie Stamm

Last Updated:

Views: 6114

Rating: 5 / 5 (60 voted)

Reviews: 83% of readers found this page helpful

Author information

Name: Stevie Stamm

Birthday: 1996-06-22

Address: Apt. 419 4200 Sipes Estate, East Delmerview, WY 05617

Phone: +342332224300

Job: Future Advertising Analyst

Hobby: Leather crafting, Puzzles, Leather crafting, scrapbook, Urban exploration, Cabaret, Skateboarding

Introduction: My name is Stevie Stamm, I am a colorful, sparkling, splendid, vast, open, hilarious, tender person who loves writing and wants to share my knowledge and understanding with you.